2021
DOI: 10.1037/xge0000926
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The development of a morality against power abuse: The case of bribery.

Abstract: In three experiments, we presented children and adults with opportunities to condemn acts resembling bribery, a prevalent form of power abuse. Adults and children (N = 333) in the United States rated the acceptability of actions by contest judges. Judges used their position in a self-serving (e.g., accepted or requested gifts from contestants prior to picking winners) versus responsible (e.g., rejected gifts, accepted gifts after judging) way. Across experiments, children by age 10 gave harsher ratings to judg… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
4
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 70 publications
1
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…As indicated by their own justifications, this shift was supported by an increase in other‐regarding concerns. These findings match previous work on children's third‐party judgments of bribes (Reyes‐Jaquez & Koenig, 2021), inequality aversion (e.g., Fehr et al, 2008), and impartiality (e.g., Mills & Keil, 2008). Interestingly, when children favored taking gifts, 43% of the time they chose as the winner the inferior drawing, indicating openness to cooperate with benefactors.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…As indicated by their own justifications, this shift was supported by an increase in other‐regarding concerns. These findings match previous work on children's third‐party judgments of bribes (Reyes‐Jaquez & Koenig, 2021), inequality aversion (e.g., Fehr et al, 2008), and impartiality (e.g., Mills & Keil, 2008). Interestingly, when children favored taking gifts, 43% of the time they chose as the winner the inferior drawing, indicating openness to cooperate with benefactors.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…Furthermore, participants in Experiment 1A, who saw the same note, usually expected that the contestant aimed to benefit from the gift (see children's explanations). Finally, children consider it worse for a judge to accept a gift before versus after picking a contest winner (Reyes‐Jaquez & Koenig, 2021). To the extent that children identify self‐interest in gifts given before picking a winner, it is likely that they are interpreting them as something more than a simple exchange.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Research with young children has also begun to make contributions to traditional topics of inquiry in political psychology such as national identity and nationalism (e.g., Brown, 2011; Hussak & Cimpian, 2019; Santhanagopalan et al, 2021; Siddiqui et al, 2020), intergroup emotions (e.g., Ran et al, 2022), and dehumanization (for review, see McLoughlin & Over, 2018). In addition, studying young children has already proved informative for the study of attitudes toward politically charged social issues such as affirmative action (e.g., Sierksma & Shutts, 2020) and corruption and crime (e.g., Dunlea & Heiphetz, 2021; McAuliffe & Dunham, 2021; Reyes‐Jaquez & Koenig, 2021).…”
Section: What Is Political Ideology?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In middle childhood, these strategic motivations further include “competitive altruism” as documented by the fact that 8‐year‐olds selectively increase their prosociality to “outshine” their peers (Herrmann et al., 2019 ). Moreover, by age 10, children understand others’ strategic motivations and evaluate those who abuse a position of power to further their own interests negatively (Reyes‐Jaquez & Koenig, 2021 ; see also Heyman et al., 2014 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%