1976
DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.83.1.37
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The cognitive side of probability learning.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

8
208
1
2

Year Published

1992
1992
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 307 publications
(224 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
8
208
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…By probability matching, we mean that if an exemplar receives Category A feedback, for instance, with probability .70, then the observer tends to classify that exemplar into Category A with probability .70. Although probability matching is often observed in probabilistic classification designs (Estes, 1976), research conducted by Ashby and his colleagues indicates that highly experienced individual observers often respond more deterministically (i.e., with probabilities closer to zero or unity) than predicted by a probability matching rule (Ashby & Gott, 1988;Ashby & Maddox, 1992). The EBRW predicts probability matching behavior in the special case in which the response criteria are set at magnitude one.…”
Section: Probabilistic Versus Deterministic Response Rules and Overalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…By probability matching, we mean that if an exemplar receives Category A feedback, for instance, with probability .70, then the observer tends to classify that exemplar into Category A with probability .70. Although probability matching is often observed in probabilistic classification designs (Estes, 1976), research conducted by Ashby and his colleagues indicates that highly experienced individual observers often respond more deterministically (i.e., with probabilities closer to zero or unity) than predicted by a probability matching rule (Ashby & Gott, 1988;Ashby & Maddox, 1992). The EBRW predicts probability matching behavior in the special case in which the response criteria are set at magnitude one.…”
Section: Probabilistic Versus Deterministic Response Rules and Overalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, a observer may learn about the strength of different sports teams by viewing the outcomes of their head-to-head competitions (Heit, Price, & Bower, 1994). Likewise, in presidential campaigns, voters gain knowledge of the strength of alternative candidates from the results of the primary elections (Estes, 1976).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Often the degree of variability of a variable is strongly distorted by contextual effects, thus affecting resulting judgments. For example, people tend to base their estimates of frequencies and probabilities on absolute vs relative frequencies (Estes, 1976) Within the knowledge engineering domain it is possible to identify and outline those judgment processes which are not normally completed effectively by the expert, knowledge engineer and the user. Statistical models have been developed or adapted but other types are possible.…”
Section: Judgment and Choice In Knowledge Engineeringmentioning
confidence: 99%