1997
DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.104.2.266
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

An exemplar-based random walk model of speeded classification.

Abstract: The authors propose and test an exemplar-based random walk model for predicting response times in tasks of speeded, multidimensional perceptual classification. The model combines elements of R.M. Nosofsky's (1986) generalized context model of categorization and G. D. Logan's (1988) instance-based model of automaticity. In the model, exemplars race among one another to be retrieved from memory, with rates determined by their similarity to test items. The retrieved exemplars provide incremental information that … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

29
984
4
2

Year Published

2002
2002
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 689 publications
(1,034 citation statements)
references
References 105 publications
29
984
4
2
Order By: Relevance
“…On the one hand, several successful models of RT assume a race between independent runners (e.g., Brown & Heathcote, 2005Logan, 1988; P. L. Smith & Van Zandt, 2000;Van Zandt, 2000b;Van Zandt, Colonius, & Proctor, 2000). On the other hand, other successful models assume competition between alternative responses, including random walk (e.g., Nosofsky & Palmeri, 1997), diffusion (e.g., Ratcliff, Van Zandt, & McKoon, 1999), and competitive leaky accumulator models (Usher & McClelland, 2001). In direct comparisons, some specific race models have not accounted for behavioral data as well as some specific competitive models (Ratcliff & Smith, 2004;Teodorescu & Usher, 2013), but in other contexts, race models sometimes do a better job of accounting for behavioral (Leite & Ratcliff, 2010) and physiological data (Ratcliff, Cherian, & Segraves, 2003; but see Purcell et al, 2010).…”
Section: General Independent Race Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…On the one hand, several successful models of RT assume a race between independent runners (e.g., Brown & Heathcote, 2005Logan, 1988; P. L. Smith & Van Zandt, 2000;Van Zandt, 2000b;Van Zandt, Colonius, & Proctor, 2000). On the other hand, other successful models assume competition between alternative responses, including random walk (e.g., Nosofsky & Palmeri, 1997), diffusion (e.g., Ratcliff, Van Zandt, & McKoon, 1999), and competitive leaky accumulator models (Usher & McClelland, 2001). In direct comparisons, some specific race models have not accounted for behavioral data as well as some specific competitive models (Ratcliff & Smith, 2004;Teodorescu & Usher, 2013), but in other contexts, race models sometimes do a better job of accounting for behavioral (Leite & Ratcliff, 2010) and physiological data (Ratcliff, Cherian, & Segraves, 2003; but see Purcell et al, 2010).…”
Section: General Independent Race Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We assume that thresholds are determined mostly by strategic factors, like expectancies of events and rewards (Ratcliff, 2006;Ratcliff & Smith, 2004). We assume that drift rates are determined partly by structural factors, like capacity limitations, the quality of stimulus information, and the quality of memory representations (Nosofsky, Little, Donkin, & Fific, 2011;Ratcliff et al, 1999), and partly by strategic factors, like division of attention among stimuli (Logan, 1996;Logan & Gordon, 2001; P. L. Smith & Ratcliff, 2009) or stimulus dimensions (Logan & Gordon, 2001;Nosofsky & Palmeri, 1997). When there is no competition for attention, we predict selective influence of experimental manipulations on model parameters: Structural factors should affect drift rates, and strategic factors should affect thresholds.…”
Section: Special Independent Race Model: the Diffusion Race Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…According to exemplar models, a category is represented by storing exemplars as they are experienced, and categorization is a function of the similarity of the target instance to some set of those stored exemplars that are retrieved for comparison (Estes, 1994;Lamberts, 1995Lamberts, , 2000Medin & Schaffer, 1978;Nosofsky, 1986;Nosofsky & Palmeri, 1997). So, to continue our example, the target hue is compared with a set of blue exemplars and a set of purple exemplars, and the target is included in the category to which its summed similarity is greatest.…”
Section: Similarity-based Models Of Categorizationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…If the prototype is a running average of previously categorized stimuli, then one can assume that the category representation will be biased toward weighting recent stimulus values more heavily. And likewise in an exemplar model, recent exemplars may be more likely to be retrieved as representative of the category or may be more heavily weighted in the computation of similarity (Nosofsky & Palmeri, 1997). The result of this overweighting of recent stimuli is that presentation of any nonfocal hue will cause the category representation to shift in the direction of that hue.…”
Section: Similarity-based Models Of Categorizationmentioning
confidence: 99%