1981
DOI: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1981.tb34459.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Clever Hans Phenomenon from an Animal Psychologist's Point of View

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

1990
1990
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 3 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Experimenter cueing is important to control because some have argued that animals can use subconscious human cues to solve cognitive tasks, as in the Clever Hans phenomenon (e.g., Beran, 2012; Hediger, 1981). Several experimental design features make this an unlikely possibility in the current experiment.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Experimenter cueing is important to control because some have argued that animals can use subconscious human cues to solve cognitive tasks, as in the Clever Hans phenomenon (e.g., Beran, 2012; Hediger, 1981). Several experimental design features make this an unlikely possibility in the current experiment.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition to the questions concerning which capacities are being examined during testing for object permanence, another question arises as to whether the procedures assess existing capacities or, instead, actually teach the concept (see Cornell, 1978;Hediger, 1981;Thomas & Walden, 1985). It is, for example, possible that (1) subjects could learn the task from observing the experimenters' behaviors, (2) the repetitions of the tasks and even the mistrials allow subjects to engage in trial-anderror learning of each task (note Dore & Dumas, 1987), or (3) the tasks provide a step-by-step program for teaching the concept.…”
Section: Effects Of Training and Learning On Success In Object-permanmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It was a productive failure because it succeeded in pointing out a possible flaw in all similar studies: the tendency to disregard the ability that experimental apes do have, which allows them to encourage the self‐deception of their experimenters. In a similar way, semiotician Thomas Sebeok, another notable critic of ape language studies, likens nonhuman sign language to a form of Clever Hans trickery (see Hediger 2010). Experimenters are so intent on communicating across human–nonhuman barriers that they overinterpret animal behavior.…”
Section: Primates Face To Facementioning
confidence: 99%