1990
DOI: 10.1016/0749-5978(90)90034-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The base-rate fallacy: Contrasting processes and outcomes of group and individual judgment

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
46
0

Year Published

1998
1998
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 66 publications
(48 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
2
46
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Yet, such comparisons have been relatively inconclusive: sometimes groups outperform individuals, sometimes their performance is comparable, and sometimes individuals outperform groups. For instance, using the very same probability judgment paradigm, Argote, Seabright, and Dyer (1986) showed that group judgments are less affected by base-rate information than judgments made by individuals; whereas Argote, Devadas, and Melone (1990) showed that sometimes group judgments are more sensitive to base-rates than individual judgments. This not-isolated example of mixed findings points to the fact that the search for a single answer to the question of whether N þ 1 heads are indeed better than one (see Hill, 1982) may be less productive, both socially and empirically, than the search for answers to the questions of when and why groups perform differently (i.e., better and worse) than individuals (see, e.g., Broadbeck & Greitemeyer, 2000;Kerr et al, 1996 for similar reasoning).…”
mentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Yet, such comparisons have been relatively inconclusive: sometimes groups outperform individuals, sometimes their performance is comparable, and sometimes individuals outperform groups. For instance, using the very same probability judgment paradigm, Argote, Seabright, and Dyer (1986) showed that group judgments are less affected by base-rate information than judgments made by individuals; whereas Argote, Devadas, and Melone (1990) showed that sometimes group judgments are more sensitive to base-rates than individual judgments. This not-isolated example of mixed findings points to the fact that the search for a single answer to the question of whether N þ 1 heads are indeed better than one (see Hill, 1982) may be less productive, both socially and empirically, than the search for answers to the questions of when and why groups perform differently (i.e., better and worse) than individuals (see, e.g., Broadbeck & Greitemeyer, 2000;Kerr et al, 1996 for similar reasoning).…”
mentioning
confidence: 94%
“…For example, groups are more susceptible to the base-rate fallacy than are individuals. 141 Another line of work shows that when groups make decisions under uncertainty (such as when physicians confer on a diagnosis or treatment option), they are more likely to discuss information shared among the members of the group than they are to discuss information known by only one member of the group. 142 Another important area for future research is an integration of the work on health communication with that on risk perception and decision making.…”
Section: Limitations and Directions For Future Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Cox and Hayne (2006) and Sutter et al (2008) even find that groups more easily fall prey to the winner's curse in common value auctions than individuals do. Moreover, Argote et al (1990) find that group discussions amplify the base rate fallacy and Kerr et al (1999) show that in many cases individual biases are amplified in groups. On the other hand, report that group decisions are closer to the rational equilibrium prediction in a centipede game, Cooper and Kagel (2005) show that groups act more strategically in signaling games and Sutter (2007) shows that groups exhibit less myopic loss aversion than individuals.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%