2011
DOI: 10.1093/brain/awr227
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

‘The anatomy underlying acute versus chronic spatial neglect’ also depends on clinical tests

Abstract: Sir,We would like to make a few comments on the interesting paper recently published in Brain by Karnath et al. (2011). We were impressed by the careful assessment of spatial neglect during acute and chronic phase, which was combined with a solid voxel-wise lesion symptom mapping technique in a series of 54 patients with right-hemisphere stroke. Anatomical data indicated that lesions in the superior and middle temporal gyri, the basal ganglia, as well as the inferior occipitofrontal fasciculus are responsible … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
62
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 56 publications
(68 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
6
62
0
Order By: Relevance
“…To avoid these prevalent intrinsic associations we refrain from using the term neglect in the present study; instead we use the term spatial inattention. To comprehensively assess (spatial) inattention, it is important to use multiple attention tests (Saj, Verdon, Vocat, & Vuilleumier, 2012;Verdon, Schwartz, Lovblad, Hauert, & Vuilleumier, 2010). Therefore, we used the line bisection test that is supposed to be sensitive to posterior lesions (at least for contralesional inattention: (Rorden, FuhrmannBerger, & Karnath, 2006;Weiss et al, 2000) as well as cancellation tests that are also affected by frontal lesions (Vossel et al, 2011).…”
Section: Accepted Manuscriptmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To avoid these prevalent intrinsic associations we refrain from using the term neglect in the present study; instead we use the term spatial inattention. To comprehensively assess (spatial) inattention, it is important to use multiple attention tests (Saj, Verdon, Vocat, & Vuilleumier, 2012;Verdon, Schwartz, Lovblad, Hauert, & Vuilleumier, 2010). Therefore, we used the line bisection test that is supposed to be sensitive to posterior lesions (at least for contralesional inattention: (Rorden, FuhrmannBerger, & Karnath, 2006;Weiss et al, 2000) as well as cancellation tests that are also affected by frontal lesions (Vossel et al, 2011).…”
Section: Accepted Manuscriptmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous studies on the association between lesion side and incidence of PVN have yielded contradictory results [8,21,22,23]. This might be explained by the heterogeneity of assessment methods [18]. Indeed, studies using discriminative cancellation tasks - known to be more sensitive - frequently report lack of significant difference in the incidence of PVN in patients with right versus left brain damage [22].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, the small sample size could have limited the possibility to identify associations using VLSM analysis (decreased power of statistical tests). Second, the statistical approach aiming to identify a single lesion focus underlying PVN does not account for the fact that visuospatial awareness is mostly supported by a network rather than a single right or left hemisphere region [6,18,24,25]. Hence, VLSM analysis, though interesting, could also be sometimes misleading.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Parietalfrontal white matter damage, involving the anterior fascicle or the superior longitudinal fascicle, can disconnect large portions of the parietal, parietal-temporal and temporal cortex from frontal areas, and thus can be involved in the pathogenesis of neglect [7]. Moreover, lesions in white matter are particularly associated with chronic neglect [8].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%