2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.052
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Temporal dynamics of prediction error processing during reward-based decision making

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

17
97
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 107 publications
(114 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
17
97
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Berns and Bell (2012) point out that even when the two dimensions of outcome feedback are presented simultaneously, participants still have to read them sequentially. Therefore, feedback processing in a task which present valence and magnitude simultaneously also contains an initial encoding stage and a follow‐up integration stage (Philiastides, Biele, Vavatzanidis, Kazzer, & Heekeren, 2010). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Berns and Bell (2012) point out that even when the two dimensions of outcome feedback are presented simultaneously, participants still have to read them sequentially. Therefore, feedback processing in a task which present valence and magnitude simultaneously also contains an initial encoding stage and a follow‐up integration stage (Philiastides, Biele, Vavatzanidis, Kazzer, & Heekeren, 2010). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We suggest that these results support our idea that the FRN amplitude is more sensitive to feedback ambiguity in the numerical context than in the symbolic indicator context; consequently, the effect of ambiguity was more prominent when the FRN was elicited by magnitude presentation than by valence presentation (second feedback phase). In line with this viewpoint, previous research has shown that compared with the P3, the FRN is more likely to reflect the characteristics of the information presented at the moment (Gu et al., 2011; Philiastides et al., 2010). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…(Chase, Swainson, Durham, Benham, & Cools, 2011;Cohen & Ranganath, 2007;Philiastides, Biele, Vavatzanidis, Kazzer, & Heekeren, 2010). Other evidence for a model-free basis for the FRN comes from the demonstration that dopamine, the neurotransmitter implicated in generating the FRN, promotes model-free rather than model-based reinforcement learning (Wunderlich, Smittenaar, & Dolan, 2012).…”
Section: Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The FRN amplitude is larger for negative feedback than for positive feedback, indicating that the FRN is sensitive to feedback valence (Gehring & Willoughby, 2002;Holroyd & Coles, 2002;Luu, Tucker, Derryberry, Reed, & Poulsen, 2003;Miltner, Braun, & Coles, 1997;Yeung, Holroyd, & Cohen, 2005). A current theory of the FRN assumes that it reflects a reinforcement signal that triggers learning from negative feedback (Holroyd & Coles, 2002), which explains why FRN amplitude (Bellebaum & Daum, 2008;Cohen & Ranganath, 2007;Philiastides, Biele, Vavatzanidis, Kazzer, & Heekeren, 2010;van der Helden, Boksem, & Blom, 2010) or oscillatory activity related to the FRN (Cavanagh, Frank, Klein, & Allen, 2010) predicts the strength of behavioral adjustments in simple decision tasks.…”
Section: Erps Related To Feedback Processingmentioning
confidence: 99%