1973
DOI: 10.1037/h0034831
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Subject-experimenter perceptions of ethical issues in human research.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
31
0

Year Published

1974
1974
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
8
1
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 59 publications
(32 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
1
31
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Annett et al (2004), in a quantitative study using asthma research participation vignettes, highlight this point when they demonstrated that parent and adolescent ratings of research benefits were often similar and that both groups inferred they would derive benefit from research procedures (e.g., placebo) even though physicians rated them as having little beneficial potential. These findings are compatible with qualitative research that has demonstrated lapses and faults in adult and adolescent comprehension of risks and benefits (Kupst 2001; Kupst et al 2003; Reynolds and Nelson, 2007; Susman, Dorn, and Fletcher 1992) and significant differences in their evaluation of research risk compared to research investigators (Michaels and Oetting 1979; Sullivan and Deiker 1973). …”
Section: The Empirical Evidence Regarding Assent To Pediatric Researchsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…Annett et al (2004), in a quantitative study using asthma research participation vignettes, highlight this point when they demonstrated that parent and adolescent ratings of research benefits were often similar and that both groups inferred they would derive benefit from research procedures (e.g., placebo) even though physicians rated them as having little beneficial potential. These findings are compatible with qualitative research that has demonstrated lapses and faults in adult and adolescent comprehension of risks and benefits (Kupst 2001; Kupst et al 2003; Reynolds and Nelson, 2007; Susman, Dorn, and Fletcher 1992) and significant differences in their evaluation of research risk compared to research investigators (Michaels and Oetting 1979; Sullivan and Deiker 1973). …”
Section: The Empirical Evidence Regarding Assent To Pediatric Researchsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…Fortunately, there is evidence that participants do not perceive deception to be unethical (Aguinis and Henle, 2001;Collins et al, 1979;Smith and Berard, 1982;Sullivan and Deiker, 1973;Wilson and Donnerstein, 1976), and debriefing seems to eliminate the negative effects of deceptive research on participants (Holmes, 1976;Smith and Richardson, 1983). Moreover, the type of deception involved when conducting a study using eLancing is defined as "mild deception," which consists of "creating false beliefs… such as misleading [participants] about the research sponsor or study purpose" (Kimmel, 2012, p. 402).…”
Section: Potential Limitations and Challenges Of Using Elancing As A mentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Fourth, most college students are generally accepting of ethically sensitive research practices such as deception and invasion of privacy (e.g., Collins, Kuhn, & King, 1979;Epstein, Suedfeld, & Silverstein, 1973;Farr & Seaver, 1975) and are less critical of those practices than members of Human Subjects Committees, psychologists, graduate students, and faculty (e.g., Korn, 1987;C. P. Smith & Berard, 1982;Sullivan & Deiker, 1973). Fifth, according to a questionnaire study by Sharpe, Adair, and Roese (1992), the continued use of deception did not evoke an increase in negative attitudes toward psychological research among the participant population.…”
Section: Does Deception Breed No Resentment?mentioning
confidence: 99%