1990
DOI: 10.3758/bf03204998
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Studies in auditory timing: 2. Rhythm patterns

Abstract: Listeners discriminated between 6-tone rhythmic patterns that differed only in the delay of the temporal position of one of the tones. On each trial, feedback was given and the subject's performance determined the amount of delay on the next trial. The 6 tones of the patterns marked off 5 intervals. In the first experiment, patterns comprised 3 "short" and 2 "long" intervals: 12121, 21121, and so forth, where the long (2) was twice the length of a short (1). In the second experiment, patterns were the compleme… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...

Citation Types

7
40
0

Year Published

1994
1994
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 61 publications
(47 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
7
40
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In these sequences, if the time interval, T, of the Weber fraction, (JND)/T, is taken as the constant IOI (where JND refers to a just noticeable time difference), then the above findings are consistent with Weber's law. Because these thresholds agree (roughly) with earlier findings based on isolated pairs of time intervals (e.g., Abel, 1972;Allan, 1979;Creelman, 1962;Getty, 1975;Small & Campbell, 1962), Monahan and Hirsh (1990) concluded that "temporal interval discrimination for tonal onsets in a sequence oftones was similar to older results for the discrimination of single temporal intervals" (p. 227). However, there is also evidence that in various nonisochronous sequences, contextual relationships related to frequency and timing properties of sequence elements influence listeners' relative threshold for discriminating a time change (Bharucha & Pryor, 1986;Hirsh et al, 1990;Jones et al, in press;Kidd & Watson, 1990;Van Noorden, 1975).…”
supporting
confidence: 79%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In these sequences, if the time interval, T, of the Weber fraction, (JND)/T, is taken as the constant IOI (where JND refers to a just noticeable time difference), then the above findings are consistent with Weber's law. Because these thresholds agree (roughly) with earlier findings based on isolated pairs of time intervals (e.g., Abel, 1972;Allan, 1979;Creelman, 1962;Getty, 1975;Small & Campbell, 1962), Monahan and Hirsh (1990) concluded that "temporal interval discrimination for tonal onsets in a sequence oftones was similar to older results for the discrimination of single temporal intervals" (p. 227). However, there is also evidence that in various nonisochronous sequences, contextual relationships related to frequency and timing properties of sequence elements influence listeners' relative threshold for discriminating a time change (Bharucha & Pryor, 1986;Hirsh et al, 1990;Jones et al, in press;Kidd & Watson, 1990;Van Noorden, 1975).…”
supporting
confidence: 79%
“…In addition, effects of context may be more evident with skilled listeners who can "make the most" of pattern structure, perhaps by extracting an especially effective temporal reference frame, T. A null hypothesis along these lines is based on the average Weber model of Monahan and Hirsh (1990), which focuses on local temporal context, namely test cycle lOIs. Because averages of test cycle lOIs (mean = 800 msec) are identical for regular and irregular sequences, if these are the primary determinant of listeners' T, then global context should have no effect on performance.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Barnes and Jones (2000) found that duration judgments about a standard and a comparison interval were most accurate when the temporal context preceding the standard had a simple harmonic relationship to the standard interval. In all of these studies, temporal context altered sensitivity to temporal information in a direction consistent with a coupled oscillator mechanism.The use of near-threshold detection measures of timing has invited comparisons between DAT and temporal information processing theories, such as the multiplelook model (Drake & Botte, 1993), the average Weber model (Monahan & Hirsh, 1990), and Povel's clock model (Povel, 1981;Povel & Essens, 1985). These models assert the distribution of a psychological duration code extracted from environmental durations, which is further asserted to relate to performance on various time perception tasks.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The use of near-threshold detection measures of timing has invited comparisons between DAT and temporal information processing theories, such as the multiplelook model (Drake & Botte, 1993), the average Weber model (Monahan & Hirsh, 1990), and Povel's clock model (Povel, 1981;Povel & Essens, 1985). These models assert the distribution of a psychological duration code extracted from environmental durations, which is further asserted to relate to performance on various time perception tasks.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%