1978
DOI: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1978.tb00776.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Students' Preferences for Information About Their Test Performance: A Social Comparison Study1

Abstract: College students received their test scores and letter grades for an actual examination and were asked to indicate their degree of interest in obtaining information (average score, highest score, etc.) in order to evaluate their performance. Overall, the students were most interested in knowing the average test score and least interested in knowing the lowest test score and the number of others who received worse test scores. There was some evidence supporting Festinger's unidirectional drive hypothesis in tha… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

4
14
0

Year Published

1989
1989
2004
2004

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 8 publications
4
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The closeness and identi®cation ®ndings provide further evidence that participants did not nominate their comparison targets at random. All these observations are consistent with previous ®ndings indicating that people often prefer comparisons with people who are performing slightly better than themselves (Arrowood & Friend, 1969;Gruder, 1971;Nosanchuk & Erickson, 1985;Suls & Tesch, 1978;Wheeler, 1966;Wilson & Benner, 1971;Ybema & Buunk, 1993) and with whom they share a meaningful identity or a close emotional bond (e.g. Miller, 1984;Miller, Turnbull, & McFarland, 1988).…”
supporting
confidence: 89%
“…The closeness and identi®cation ®ndings provide further evidence that participants did not nominate their comparison targets at random. All these observations are consistent with previous ®ndings indicating that people often prefer comparisons with people who are performing slightly better than themselves (Arrowood & Friend, 1969;Gruder, 1971;Nosanchuk & Erickson, 1985;Suls & Tesch, 1978;Wheeler, 1966;Wilson & Benner, 1971;Ybema & Buunk, 1993) and with whom they share a meaningful identity or a close emotional bond (e.g. Miller, 1984;Miller, Turnbull, & McFarland, 1988).…”
supporting
confidence: 89%
“…Downward comparison was not an important feature of information seeking in the students in this study. Suls & Tesch (1978) reported a similar lack of interest in worse-off others. Closer investigation of subjects' reasons for such lack of downward comparison would help to resolve whether students were avoiding embarrassment (Brickman c)r Bulman, 1977), or whether they did not anticipate that such information would be useful.…”
Section: Margaret Foddy and Lan Crunlflmentioning
confidence: 84%
“…As was noted earlier, many rankorder studies demonstrate that people tend to compare themselves with others whose scores are better than their own (Arrowood & Friend, 1969;Friend & Gilbert, 1973;Gruder, 1971;Samuel, 1973;Suls & Tesch, 1978;Thornton & Arrowood, 1966;Wheeler et al, 1969;Wheeler & Koestner, 1984). When comparing themselves with the extremes of the dimension, their first choice is the most positive extreme rather than the most negative (e.g.,, Arrowood & Friend, 1969), and even when they compare themselves with similar others, they compare themselves with those who are close but "above" them in the rank order rather than "below" them (e.g., Wheeler et al, 1969).…”
Section: Target Selections Along the Dimension Under Evaluationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This interpretation of range seeking is consistent with evidence from studies that do not use the rank-order method. When subjects are not constrained to select a single score, they appear to be interested in obtaining any information they can about the dimension; they are interested in learning the group's mean (Sanders, 1982a;Suls & Tesch, 1978) or in examining the entire distribution of scores (Brickman & Berman, 1971), especially when they are uncertain about the dimension under evaluation (Brickman & Berman, 1971;Gruder et al, 1975). These results suggest that on unfamiliar dimensions, or on previously familiar dimensions in a new social context, one's goal is to learn the distribution of others' standings, perhaps as an initial step toward self-evaluation (cf.…”
Section: Target Selections Along the Dimension Under Evaluationmentioning
confidence: 99%