2019
DOI: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000363
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Strategies to Improve Name Learning

Abstract: Abstract. The following points emerge from the present review of strategies to improve the learning of proper names: (a) Face-name mnemonic techniques based on mental imagery have been shown to be efficient in laboratory settings in both young and older adults. Unfortunately, they are particularly effortful and require capacity for imagination, making them difficult to apply in a real conversational context. (b) Strategies based on spaced retrieval practice have been found to be efficient both in laboratory an… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Spaced retrieval is a prototype of a simple but effective strategy for everyday remembering. It is a testing-based strategy that has been shown to be highly effective for improving older adults’ remembering, including learning new names ( Brédart, 2019 ). Spaced retrieval involves repeated retrieval of information, linked to either explicit or implicit cues.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Spaced retrieval is a prototype of a simple but effective strategy for everyday remembering. It is a testing-based strategy that has been shown to be highly effective for improving older adults’ remembering, including learning new names ( Brédart, 2019 ). Spaced retrieval involves repeated retrieval of information, linked to either explicit or implicit cues.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Traditional memory training intervention studies with older adults work well in training memory strategies, but have had limited success in terms of transfer of training (e.g., Simons et al, 2016 ). Often training focuses on complicated mnemonics that can be burdensome to learn and difficult to actually use in real life ( Brédart, 2019 ; Brooks et al, 1999 ; Derwinger et al, 2005 ; McKitrick et al, 1999 ). In particular, the evidence for improvement in everyday memory following memory strategy training is both scant and limited primarily to subjective memory ( Hudes et al, 2019 ).…”
Section: Background and Objectivesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, people need to have a set of metacognitive skills to use in real-world situations. For instance, training spaced retrieval ( Brédart, 2019 ; Camp, 2006 ; Walmsley & Fuqua, 2018 ; Wiegand et al, 2013 ) allows one to decide to learn someone’s name at a social gathering and access an effective tool for doing so. Second, we focus on creating a set of “habits-of-mind” ( Stine-Morrow, 2007 ) that increase the likelihood of effective use of existing and newly trained memory aids.…”
Section: Background and Objectivesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Learning people’s names is another example of paired-associate learning of faces and names. Keywords and face–name imagery have been devised for mnemonics that facilitate paired-associate learning in foreign-language vocabulary learning and in face–name learning (Atkinson, 1975; Brédart, 2019; Morris et al, 1978). This will be addressed later in this study.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, the use of retrieval practice raises another concern. Although the retrieval-practice method is recognised to be very good for facilitating paired-associate learning (Brédart, 2019; Landauer & Bjork, 1978), the method is less suitable for examining the effects of context. In the varied condition, retrieval of response terms is minimally assisted by the context presented with the pairs at study, because the contexts varied every learning session.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%