2003
DOI: 10.3758/bf03196115
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Stimulus-related priming during task switching

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

2
25
0

Year Published

2004
2004
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
2
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Rogers and Monsell also found that this switch cost decreased when the interval between one stimulus and the next (the response-to-stimulus interval, or RSI) was increased but that there was an irreducible switch cost that was not removed, no matter how long RSI became. Other researchers have since replicated and extended these findings, using the repeated runs design (e.g., De Jong, 2000;Gilbert & Shallice, 2002;Karayanidis, Coltheart, Michie, & Murphy, 2003;Lien, Schweickert, & Proctor, 2003;Los, 1999;Nieuwenhuis & Monsell, 2002;Sohn & Anderson, 2003;Yeung & Monsell, 2003).…”
mentioning
confidence: 85%
“…Rogers and Monsell also found that this switch cost decreased when the interval between one stimulus and the next (the response-to-stimulus interval, or RSI) was increased but that there was an irreducible switch cost that was not removed, no matter how long RSI became. Other researchers have since replicated and extended these findings, using the repeated runs design (e.g., De Jong, 2000;Gilbert & Shallice, 2002;Karayanidis, Coltheart, Michie, & Murphy, 2003;Lien, Schweickert, & Proctor, 2003;Los, 1999;Nieuwenhuis & Monsell, 2002;Sohn & Anderson, 2003;Yeung & Monsell, 2003).…”
mentioning
confidence: 85%
“…This dissociation suggests that generic preparation reflects the basic functional process, in which case facilitation of this process by repetition priming seems like an obvious candidate account of switch cost. Priming, or some related automatic or passive process, is responsible for some amount of switch cost, as measured both with cuing paradigms (Arrington, Altmann, & Carr, 2003;Meiran, Chorev, & Sapir, 2000;Sohn & Anderson, 2001 and with alternating runs (Koch, 2003;Ruthruff, Remington, & Johnston, 2001;Yeung & Monsell, 2003), and although priming does not eliminate switch cost with longer intertrial intervals (Sohn & Anderson, 2003), it can last for minutes (Allport & Wylie, 2000;Waszak, Hommel, & Allport, 2003) or in general for days (e.g., Buck-Gengler & Healy, 2001), so a priming account is by no means ruled out. In the end, a priming account of switch cost, even if it must assume action at longer temporal intervals, may be more parsimonious than the reconfiguration view (Monsell, 2003), which requires a brand new mechanism to be added to the inventory just to explain switch cost.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Koch (2001, as noted above) and Arrington (2002, Experiment 2) both manipulated SOA between subjects and found no switch preparation. Sohn and Anderson (2003) manipulated SOA between subjects and found switch preparation, but crossed their manipulation with a within-subjects manipulation of task "foreknowledge"-on alternating blocks, participants received no task cue before stimulus onset. Thus, the opportunity to prepare during the SOA was effectively nullified on alternating blocks, so this study may not contradict the notion that between-subjects SOA manipulations fail to produce switch preparation.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, many authors consider priming itself as a possible underlying mechanism for explaining task switch costs (e.g., Allport, Styles, & Hsieh, 1994;Sohn & Anderson, 2003). For this reason, disentangling the two mechanisms may be very difficult and even impossible because evidence for the strategy switch cost mechanism does not exclude the priming mechanism.…”
Section: Towards An Explanation Of the Perseveration Effectmentioning
confidence: 99%