2015
DOI: 10.3758/s13415-015-0355-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Stimulus conflict triggers behavioral avoidance

Abstract: According to a recent extension of the conflictmonitoring theory, conflict between two competing response tendencies is registered as an aversive event and triggers a motivation to avoid the source of conflict. In the present study, we tested this assumption. Over five experiments, we examined whether conflict is associated with an avoidance motivation and whether stimulus conflict or response conflict triggers an avoidance tendency. Participants first performed a color Stroop task. In a subsequent motivation … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

3
28
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 47 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 75 publications
3
28
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The observation that conflict adaptation is modulated by the length of the ITI is not odd, but accords well with previous studies in the literature: the CAE is usually most pronounced using short ITIs and tends to decay sharply when using longer intervals Alternatively, the implementation of enhanced conflict-driven adjustment during the encounter of integral negative emotion could be regarded as a form of emotion regulation (Dignath & Eder, 2015;Dreisbach & Fischer, 2015), as opposed to cognitive control per se. Although remaining largely speculative at this stage, it is feasible that this enhanced adaptive control triggered by integral negative emotion at a short ITI might reflect a compensatory mechanism, whereby participants would actively try to reduce these negative experiences or feelings in this condition (Gyurak, Gross, & Etkin, 2011;Dignath & Eder, 2015;Inzlicht et al, 2015;Schouppe, De Houwer, Ridderinkhof, & Notebaert, 2012). Such an interpretation accords well with the affect alarm model of self-control, in which negative information can be viewed as adaptive to some extent (i.e., when low to mild doses of negative emotion are used with healthy participants), acting like a guiding signal that there is something wrong happening in the environment somehow, and an adjustment or compensation is therefore timely required to overcome (and change) this feeling (Inzlicht, Bartholow, & Hirsh, 2013;.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…The observation that conflict adaptation is modulated by the length of the ITI is not odd, but accords well with previous studies in the literature: the CAE is usually most pronounced using short ITIs and tends to decay sharply when using longer intervals Alternatively, the implementation of enhanced conflict-driven adjustment during the encounter of integral negative emotion could be regarded as a form of emotion regulation (Dignath & Eder, 2015;Dreisbach & Fischer, 2015), as opposed to cognitive control per se. Although remaining largely speculative at this stage, it is feasible that this enhanced adaptive control triggered by integral negative emotion at a short ITI might reflect a compensatory mechanism, whereby participants would actively try to reduce these negative experiences or feelings in this condition (Gyurak, Gross, & Etkin, 2011;Dignath & Eder, 2015;Inzlicht et al, 2015;Schouppe, De Houwer, Ridderinkhof, & Notebaert, 2012). Such an interpretation accords well with the affect alarm model of self-control, in which negative information can be viewed as adaptive to some extent (i.e., when low to mild doses of negative emotion are used with healthy participants), acting like a guiding signal that there is something wrong happening in the environment somehow, and an adjustment or compensation is therefore timely required to overcome (and change) this feeling (Inzlicht, Bartholow, & Hirsh, 2013;.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Therefore, besides the context defined by the frequency of incongruent trials and the use of evaluative feedback to reinforce it (or the lack thereof), inter-individual differences in specific dispositions, here with a focus on emotion regulation and more specifically reappraisal eventually combined to determine the engagement of proactive vs. reactive control strategies to deal with conflict (Braver et al, 2007). More generally, our new results are compatible with recent data and models available in the literature arguing that emotion regulation, besides conflict processing per se, can dynamically influence CC (Dignath & Eder, 2015;Dreisbach & Fischer, 2015). In this context, it appears therefore important to consider and model carefully inter-individual differences in emotion regulation, or other dispositions, when exploring this fundamental process as they appear to influence it substantially (see also Braver, 2012;Braver, Cole, & Yarkoni, 2010;Egner, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…Negative emotion might provide another source triggering enhanced control adjustments, given the close ties between 7 defensive motivation and CC (Inzlicht et al, 2015). From this affective perspective, it is reasonable to conceive that conflict-driven adaptive control processes could be regarded as a form of emotion regulation to some extent (Dignath & Eder, 2015;Dreisbach & Fischer, 2015). According to a dominant model in the affective sciences literature (Gross, 2002), emotion regulation can be divided into different processes or strategies, including suppression and cognitive reappraisal.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the wake of Botvinick (2007)’s proposal, a growing number of behavioral studies confirmed its first assumption, namely that cognitive conflicts appear to be experienced as aversive events (Brouillet, Ferrier, Grosselin, & Brouillet, 2011; Dignath & Eder, 2015; Dreisbach & Fischer, 2012; Fritz & Dreisbach, 2013, 2015; Schouppe et al, 2012; 2015; for a review, see Dreisbach & Fischer, 2015). For example, Dreisbach and Fischer (2012) investigated this by combining a classic color Stroop task (Stroop, 1935) with an affective priming paradigm (Fazio, 2001).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%