1999
DOI: 10.1006/jesp.1998.1373
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Stereotype Threat and Women's Math Performance

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

82
2,393
21
72

Year Published

2002
2002
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2,696 publications
(2,627 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
82
2,393
21
72
Order By: Relevance
“…All three aspects, moreover, can involve feedback loops. When the feedback runs from teacher bias to lowered self-evaluation, reduced effort and subsequent lower academic performance, the loop involves what Farkas et al termed a "self-fulfilling prophecy" and which in the more recent literature has been termed a "stereotype threat" (Spencer et al, 1999). When the feedback instead runs through a heightened ability to perform in a "socially-enhanced learning environment" that uses "well-rounded" academic evaluation criteria, the mechanism is the "cumulative exposure" form of a cumulative advantage process (DiPrete and Eirich, 2006).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…All three aspects, moreover, can involve feedback loops. When the feedback runs from teacher bias to lowered self-evaluation, reduced effort and subsequent lower academic performance, the loop involves what Farkas et al termed a "self-fulfilling prophecy" and which in the more recent literature has been termed a "stereotype threat" (Spencer et al, 1999). When the feedback instead runs through a heightened ability to perform in a "socially-enhanced learning environment" that uses "well-rounded" academic evaluation criteria, the mechanism is the "cumulative exposure" form of a cumulative advantage process (DiPrete and Eirich, 2006).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research on the impact of stereotype threat, or the risk of being judged by negative stereotypes of the ability of women in maths and science (e.g. Spencer et al, 1999) do support the idea that female astronomers (on average) would tend to find it harder to ask questions, for example (Carr and Steele, 2010) found that women facing stereotype threat are more risk averse than typical. In addition, there are suggestions that subtle differences in criticism from teachers over time can lead girls/women to develop low self confidence in their abilities, while boys/men may (again on average) overestimate theirs (e.g.…”
Section: The Psychology Of Asking Questionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Steinpreis et al 1999), as well as stereotype threat (Betz et al, 2014;Spencer et al, 1999) and the sheer fact of the raw numbers can still make the astronomical community feel like a hostile place for women, despite the best intentions of all involved.…”
Section: B Implications For Uk Astronomymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although we are not aware of any systematic investigation of chess-related gender stereotypes, such stereotypes are easily visible in chess magazines and have been discussed in detail in Shahade's (2005) popular book. Applying the stereotype threat model (Steele & Aronson, 1995;Spencer, Steele, & Quinn, 1999), the activation of gender stereotypes during a tournament is likely to interfere with the chess performance of women, presumably due to their preoccupation to confirm negative expectations. The stereotype threat model has received considerable empirical support both in academic domains (Inzlicht & Good, 2006;Maass & Cadinu, 2003;Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002) and sports (Stone, Lynch, Sjomeling, & Darley, 1999; for a review see Beilock & McConnell, 2004), but, to our knowledge, has never been investigated as a potential explanation of the gender gap in chess.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%