2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.specom.2021.04.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Speech rhythm convergence in a dyadic reading task

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
2
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
1
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…in which the duration between consecutive behaviors is always the same) are often present in music, less strict periodicities can be observed in different chorusing behaviors (9) and styles of speech (47), especially if they involve a social context. For example, when dyads read out loud, the temporal regularity of their speech increases (48,49). Our results provide new evidence on these types of activity patterns and show that spontaneous conversational activities in small groups can exhibit both bursty (type I) and isochronous (type II) dynamics at the aggregate level.…”
Section: Coarse-grained Metrics Of Collective Rhythm B Metricsupporting
confidence: 51%
“…in which the duration between consecutive behaviors is always the same) are often present in music, less strict periodicities can be observed in different chorusing behaviors (9) and styles of speech (47), especially if they involve a social context. For example, when dyads read out loud, the temporal regularity of their speech increases (48,49). Our results provide new evidence on these types of activity patterns and show that spontaneous conversational activities in small groups can exhibit both bursty (type I) and isochronous (type II) dynamics at the aggregate level.…”
Section: Coarse-grained Metrics Of Collective Rhythm B Metricsupporting
confidence: 51%
“…For example, studies on the rhythmic characteristics of infantcompared to adult-directed speech have shown that: a) English, Catalan and Spanish mothers present less durational variability of consonantal and vocalic intervals as well as longer vowel duration when speaking to their children compared to addressing adults (Payne et al, 2009); b) in Australian English delta modulations corresponding to the prosodic stress is greater in infant-than in adult-directed speech, while theta modulations, tracking syllable patterns, dominated the adult-directed speech modulation spectrum (Leong et al, 2017). Not only do speech rhythm vary depending on the interlocutors' characteristics, but the presence itself of an interlocutor (i.e., reading partner) has been shown to influence the degree of rhythm entrainment in synchronous reading tasks (Cerda-Oñate et al, 2021). In light of these findings, it seems plausible to assume that speakers can also mutually adapt the production of segmental timing features after exposure to a dialogue partner.…”
Section: Rhythmic Accommodationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We argue that by being attuned to the other's actions, and by regulating self-generated action to engage with those actions, the outcome is a meeting of minds that is a basic, automatic, and non-reflexive understanding of the other's mental transitional states. This dynamic attuning with the other can happen with any behaviour, such as when mimicking gestures and mannerisms, as in the so-called "chameleon effect" (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999), or when copying the intonation and prosodic patterns of the other while reading together (Cerda-Oñate et al, 2021). Also, the reciprocal dialectical exchange does not need to share the same sensory modality (e.g.…”
Section: Afterthoughtsmentioning
confidence: 99%