1994
DOI: 10.1159/000261962
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Some Distributional Facts about Fricatives and a Perceptual Explanation

Abstract: Across and within languages voiced sibilants tend to be disfavored relative to voiceless ones. This paper explores the claim that voicing more adversely affects the distinctive acoustic properties of sibilants than those of nonsibilants. One prediction associated with this claim is that voicing differentially lowers the amplitude of frication noise for sibilants and non-sibiliants so that amplitude differences between the two classes are reduced. Acoustic measurements confirm this prediction. A second predicti… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
14
0

Year Published

1995
1995
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 8 publications
0
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This difficulty in determining the defining acoustic properties has been mirrored at the perceptual level. Among fricatives, /f/ and /T/ and /v/ and /D/ are most easily confused (e.g., Balise and Diehl, 1994;Jongman and Wang, submitted). Given the reported difficulty in recognition of these fricatives, Miller and Nicely (1955) hypothesized that the distinction between /f/ and /T/ and /v/ and /D/ may be based on non-acoustic information:…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This difficulty in determining the defining acoustic properties has been mirrored at the perceptual level. Among fricatives, /f/ and /T/ and /v/ and /D/ are most easily confused (e.g., Balise and Diehl, 1994;Jongman and Wang, submitted). Given the reported difficulty in recognition of these fricatives, Miller and Nicely (1955) hypothesized that the distinction between /f/ and /T/ and /v/ and /D/ may be based on non-acoustic information:…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This would mean that the aperiodic pitch impressions of voiceless fricatives either remain approximately constant for each fricative and speaker or that they vary randomly across different contexts, particularly those contexts of intonation and tone. This is quite an assumption given that (a) about 25% of the speech signal in, for example, English everyday conversation is voiceless, that (b) almost half of this percentage is due to voiceless fricatives [22], and that (c) languages have more voiceless fricatives than voiceless stops (according to the UPSID database, [23]). …”
Section: Background: the Notion Of "Segmental Intonation"mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Balise and Diehl (1994) measured the RMS amplitude of frication noise in English sibilants and nonsibilants and found that voicing effectively reduces the frication noise of sibilants so that they become less distinguishable from their nonsibilant counterparts. Perceptual experiments also verified this by showing that voiced fricatives are more difficult to identify than voiceless ones, and the impact is more severe on the perception of sibilants than nonsibilants (Balise and Diehl 1994;Miller and Nicely 1955;Singh and Black 1966;Wang and Bilger 1973).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%