2014
DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00354
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Social relevance drives viewing behavior independent of low-level salience in rhesus macaques

Abstract: Quantifying attention to social stimuli during the viewing of complex social scenes with eye tracking has proven to be a sensitive method in the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders years before average clinical diagnosis. Rhesus macaques provide an ideal model for understanding the mechanisms underlying social viewing behavior, but to date no comparable behavioral task has been developed for use in monkeys. Using a novel scene-viewing task, we monitored the gaze of three rhesus macaques while they freely vi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

1
9
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 112 publications
1
9
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Whereas the bias reduction in experiment 1 may therefore be explained by the general tendency to perceive a person facing oneself as more close than a person looking away (Jung et al 2016), and perhaps as more close than the object used here, there was no bias reduction but still a reduction in position error in experiment 2, which in our view cannot be explained by such a systematic shift towards the self. Our results may therefore indicate a social advantage in spatial encoding, similar as has been observed in other contexts, such as attentional capture for faces (Weaver and Lauwereyns 2011), attentional capture for social stimuli (Gluckman and Johnson 2013), enhanced tactile spatial perception when seeing a hand (Kennett et al 2001), or a social preference when observing complex scenes (Solyst and Buffalo 2014).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Whereas the bias reduction in experiment 1 may therefore be explained by the general tendency to perceive a person facing oneself as more close than a person looking away (Jung et al 2016), and perhaps as more close than the object used here, there was no bias reduction but still a reduction in position error in experiment 2, which in our view cannot be explained by such a systematic shift towards the self. Our results may therefore indicate a social advantage in spatial encoding, similar as has been observed in other contexts, such as attentional capture for faces (Weaver and Lauwereyns 2011), attentional capture for social stimuli (Gluckman and Johnson 2013), enhanced tactile spatial perception when seeing a hand (Kennett et al 2001), or a social preference when observing complex scenes (Solyst and Buffalo 2014).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…It has been argued before that visual body perception triggers attentional focus (Weaver and Lauwereyns 2011;Gluckman and Johnson 2013;Solyst and Buffalo 2014), perhaps via attentional prioritization (Truong and Todd 2016). Attentional prioritization also modulates hippocampal networks, and the PPC (Cordova et al 2016;Levichkina et al 2017).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Macaque infants are a promising NHP model, given their similarities with human infants, including strong mother–infant bonds, complex social interactions, and dedicated neural systems for social information processing (Shepherd & Freiwald, 2018). As in humans, adult macaques display privileged processing of social compared to nonsocial stimuli (Machado, Whitaker, Smith, Patterson, & Bauman, 2015; Nakata, Eifuku, & Tamura, 2018; Solyst & Buffalo, 2014; Taubert, Wardle, Flessert, Leopold, & Ungerleider, 2017), and infant macaque social attention is positively correlated with later social development. For example, male infant macaques display an increase social attention between 1 and 6 months of age, especially attention to the eye region of faces, and this increase is associated with more prosocial peer interactions between 3 and 18 months of age (Ryan et al., in press).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Importantly, prior research also shed light on the power of saliency-based predictions in the context of social information. For example, several studies yielded support that the influence of physical saliency on gazing behavior may be especially weak for socially relevant stimuli (e.g., Nyström and Holmqvist, 2008; Birmingham et al, 2009a,b; Fletcher-Watson et al, 2009; Zwickel and Vo, 2010; Hall et al, 2011; Scheller et al, 2012; Suda and Kitazawa, 2015; see also Kano and Tomonaga, 2011; Solyst and Buffalo, 2014) and a number of modeling approaches provided evidence that further sources of information (e.g., locations of faces) may be considered in addition to low-level saliency (e.g., Cerf et al, 2008, 2009; Marat et al, 2013; Xu et al, 2014; Parks et al, 2015; for a review, see Tatler et al, 2011). However, the attentional influence of physical saliency in the context of social information has so far not been characterized sufficiently.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%