2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2016.05.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Social exclusion, self-affirmation, and health information avoidance

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
24
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
1
24
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These findings indicate that the affirmation of values can mute the physiological response to cognitive conflicts following from expectancy violations such as task error and perceptual anomalies. In contrast, studies have shown that self-affirmation has no buffer effect on the negative effects of ostracism (Dingwall, 2011;Howell & Shepperd, 2016; and see Williams, 2009). Consequently, if the pupillary reactivity in response to cues of ostracism similarly reflects a conflict detection process, then we should expect a muted pupillary response as a function of value affirmation, which would support the hypothesis that pupillary reactivity following ostracism cues reflects an underlying conflict detection mechanism.…”
Section: Studymentioning
confidence: 97%
“…These findings indicate that the affirmation of values can mute the physiological response to cognitive conflicts following from expectancy violations such as task error and perceptual anomalies. In contrast, studies have shown that self-affirmation has no buffer effect on the negative effects of ostracism (Dingwall, 2011;Howell & Shepperd, 2016; and see Williams, 2009). Consequently, if the pupillary reactivity in response to cues of ostracism similarly reflects a conflict detection process, then we should expect a muted pupillary response as a function of value affirmation, which would support the hypothesis that pupillary reactivity following ostracism cues reflects an underlying conflict detection mechanism.…”
Section: Studymentioning
confidence: 97%
“…On the one hand, severe perils may prompt greater avoidance of risk information to the extent that people view the peril as overly threatening, demanding more coping resources than they have available. As we noted earlier, research in health finds that people display greater avoidance when they have few resources or when the resource demands are high (Howell et al, ; Howell & Shepperd, , ). On the other hand, severe perils may prompt less avoidance of risk information to the extent that people view the peril as too important to ignore.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 68%
“…Second, people are more likely to avoid information about a threat if they lack resources to address the threat. For example, studies show that people with fewer resources to cope with bad news display greater avoidance of health information (Howell, Crosier, & Shepperd, ; Howell & Shepperd, , ). Preparing for an environmental peril, such as an approaching hurricane, requires time and money to shop for supplies, prepare one's home, and plan an evacuation route.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…": 1 = not able to pay the bill; 2 = able to pay, but with difficulty; or 3 = able to pay comfortably. We scored the financial security measure using recommend procedures (Shepperd et al, 2017). Higher scores indicated greater financial security.…”
Section: Resourcesmentioning
confidence: 99%