2000
DOI: 10.1002/1099-0720(200009)14:5<421::aid-acp659>3.0.co;2-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Social demand characteristics in children's and adults' eyewitness memory and suggestibility: the effect of different interviewers on free recall and recognition

Abstract: Shortly after viewing a video of a theft, 5-and 7-year-old children and adults were interviewed with free recall and either misleading or unbiased-leading questions. After a 2-day delay, participants were interviewed with free recall and recognition questions administered by either the same or a dierent interviewer. Results from day 1 replicate previous ®ndings with levels of recall and resistance to suggestibility increasing with age. Counter to predictions, correct recognition performance on day 2 was greate… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
52
0
2

Year Published

2004
2004
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 64 publications
(58 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
(39 reference statements)
4
52
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Most importantly, the RES effect persisted for peripheral items even when both the initial and final tests were administered in a free recall format. Similar to previous reports, the free recall format is highly resistant to the influence of misinformation (Bjorklund et al, 1998(Bjorklund et al, , 2000, such that the non-tested participants recalled very little of the peripheral misinformation they encountered (M = .10). Remarkably, initial testing nearly doubled false recall probability for these items (M = .19), t(118) = 2.57, d = .47, p < .02 (refer to Figure 3).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 77%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Most importantly, the RES effect persisted for peripheral items even when both the initial and final tests were administered in a free recall format. Similar to previous reports, the free recall format is highly resistant to the influence of misinformation (Bjorklund et al, 1998(Bjorklund et al, , 2000, such that the non-tested participants recalled very little of the peripheral misinformation they encountered (M = .10). Remarkably, initial testing nearly doubled false recall probability for these items (M = .19), t(118) = 2.57, d = .47, p < .02 (refer to Figure 3).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 77%
“…Further, despite the vast literature on eyewitness suggestibility, a surprisingly small number of studies have examined the influence of misinformation on free recall performance. Of the few studies that used free recall as the final test, some have failed to demonstrate any discernable misinformation effect (Bjorklund, Bjorklund, Brown, & Cassel, 1998;Bjorklund, Cassell, Bjorklund, et al, 2000). Therefore, whether initial testing can produce confidently held false memories for misinformation that persist in a free recall final test remains to be seen (cf., Roediger & McDermott, 1995).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In some cases, all the interviews with a child were conducted by the same interviewer, but in other cases they were conducted by different interviewers and from different professional groups. The limited research suggests that children are more accurate in second interviews if they are interviewed by the same person as in the first interview (Bjorklund et al, 2000). However, a comparison was not possible in the present study due to the small sample size.…”
Section: Limitations and Further Researchmentioning
confidence: 77%
“…For children questioned repeatedly, the same interviewer conducted each interview to eliminate effects of changing interviewers on performance (e.g., Bjorklund et al, 2000;Quas & Schaaf, 2002). During the play event and interviews, the presence of male research assistants (RAs) was prohibited in the laboratory, and no men interacted with the children.…”
Section: Materials and Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%