2014
DOI: 10.1037/xap0000001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Retrieval enhances eyewitness suggestibility to misinformation in free and cued recall.

Abstract: Immediately recalling a witnessed event can increase people's susceptibility to later postevent misinformation. But this retrieval-enhanced suggestibility (RES) effect has been shown only when the initial recall test included specific questions that reappeared on the final test. Moreover, it is unclear whether this phenomenon is affected by the centrality of event details. These limitations make it difficult to generalize RES to criminal investigations, which often begin with free recall prior to more specific… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
51
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

4
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 43 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 76 publications
(117 reference statements)
4
51
1
Order By: Relevance
“…First, initial retrieval accuracy was far higher (~89% of questions were recalled correctly) in Saunders and MacLeod's study than was typically observed in studies that reported RES (~55% initial test accuracy). This is important because highly recallable items (i.e., easy items, central items) are more resistant to the RES effect than items that are more difficult to remember (e.g., peripheral items, Chan & Langley, 2011;Wilford, Chan, & Tuhn, 2012). It may be that Saunders and MacLeod's critical items were easily remembered and thus not susceptible to the RES effect.…”
Section: Testing and Suggestibilitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, initial retrieval accuracy was far higher (~89% of questions were recalled correctly) in Saunders and MacLeod's study than was typically observed in studies that reported RES (~55% initial test accuracy). This is important because highly recallable items (i.e., easy items, central items) are more resistant to the RES effect than items that are more difficult to remember (e.g., peripheral items, Chan & Langley, 2011;Wilford, Chan, & Tuhn, 2012). It may be that Saunders and MacLeod's critical items were easily remembered and thus not susceptible to the RES effect.…”
Section: Testing and Suggestibilitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…RES has been shown using a variety of materials, including video materials ranging from about 5 min (Wilford, Chan, & Tuhn, 2014) to 40 min in length (Chan et al, 2009). Researchers have also demonstrated RES using slideshows instead of videos (Rindal et al, 2016).…”
Section: Encoding Variablesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Here, we refer to this idea as the borrowed time hypothesis. Specifically, we argue that testing participants on the name (with or without feedback) leads them to perseverate on learning the name, either because receiving a test on an item increases its perceived importance (Wilford, Chan, & Tuhn, 2014;Dirkx, Thoma, Kester, & Kirschner, 2014), its perceived probability of being tested again later ), or because being tested on a item highlights its difficulty (Kang, 2010). As a result, participants continue to study the name (when feedback is given) or continue to attempt retrieval of the name (when feedback is not given) when they were supposed to 1 Specifically, Finn and Roediger noted that "This impairment did not appear to be due to selective attention to the feedback because no significant differences emerged between the tests with feedback and the test without feedback conditions in the recall of professions" (p. 1669).…”
Section: Finn and Roediger Concluded That "A Clear Theoretical Accounmentioning
confidence: 99%