1974
DOI: 10.1007/bf00918886
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Skin conductance, reaction time, and observational ratings in learning-disabled children

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

1978
1978
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 6 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Two studies (Boydstun et al, 1968;Spring et al, 1974) reported that hyperkinetic subjects required fewer trials to reach a criterion of complete habituation than did controls, and a third (Cohen & Douglas, 1972) found a nonsignificant trend in the same direction. Furthermore, Rugel and Rosenthal (1974) obtained a significant Group X Trial interaction that indicated a more rapid rate of SCR habituation in their learning-disabled group than in controls. They also found that learning-disabled subjects rapidly declined in basal SCL during nontask periods but that controls exhibited no similar "tonic habituation.…”
Section: Autonomic Arousalmentioning
confidence: 92%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Two studies (Boydstun et al, 1968;Spring et al, 1974) reported that hyperkinetic subjects required fewer trials to reach a criterion of complete habituation than did controls, and a third (Cohen & Douglas, 1972) found a nonsignificant trend in the same direction. Furthermore, Rugel and Rosenthal (1974) obtained a significant Group X Trial interaction that indicated a more rapid rate of SCR habituation in their learning-disabled group than in controls. They also found that learning-disabled subjects rapidly declined in basal SCL during nontask periods but that controls exhibited no similar "tonic habituation.…”
Section: Autonomic Arousalmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…A number of studies have compared tonic skin conductance levels (SCLs) of subjects classified as hyperkinetic, minimally brain dysfunctional, and learning disabled with groups of matched controls. Only one study (Satterfield & Dawson, 1971) found that hyperkinetic subjects have significantly lower SCLs than controls, and five studies failed to find SCL differences between controls and children with MBD (Boydstun, Ackerman, Stevens, Clements, Peters, & Dykman, 1968;Zahn, Abate, Little, & Wender, 197S), children with learning disabilities (Rugel & Rosenthal, 1974), hyperkinetics (Cohen & Douglas, 1972), or hyperkinetics who respond positively to methylphenidate (Spring, Greenberg, Scott, & Hopwood, 1974). Similarly, two studies reported that hyperkinetic children have significantly lower base rates (Conners, 1975;Forges, Walker, Korb, & Sprague, 1975), and two studies reported no heart rate differences between hy-perkinetic subjects and controls (Boydstun et al, 1968;Zahn et al, 1975).…”
Section: Autonomic Arousalmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The presence or absence of attention deficits cannot be used as a basis for differentiation between these disorders. Poorer selective attention (Ross, 1976), greater distractibility (Hebben et al 1981), slower reaction times (Dykman, Walls, Suzuki, Ackerman & Peters, 1970;Rugel & Rosenthal, 1974), and poorer sustained attention (Doyle et al, 1976;Dykman et al, 1979) have all been documented in learning disabled children. Sanson (1984) attempted to identify differences between purely LD, hyperactive LD and hyperactive non-LD children on a range of tasks including auditory and visual attention.…”
Section: Differentiation Between Disordersmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The attention characteristics of LD and ADD-H children have also been investigated using a habituation task. Studies that have compared the performances of LD, ADD-H and control children on this task have reported that LD and ADD-H children exhibit a more rapid rate of response reduction than normal children (e.g., Rugel & Rosenthal, 1974;Spring, Greenberg, Scott, & Hopwood, 1974).…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%