2007
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.5.931
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Shared syntactic representations in bilinguals: Evidence for the role of word-order repetition.

Abstract: Studies on syntactic priming strongly suggest that bilinguals can store a single integrated representation of constructions that are similar in both languages (e.g., Spanish and English passives; R. J. Hartsuiker, M. J. Pickering, & E. Veltkamp, 2004). However, they may store 2 separate representations of constructions that involve different word orders (e.g., German and English passives; H. Loebell & K. Bock, 2003). In 5 experiments, the authors investigated within--and between--languages priming of Dutch, En… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

23
218
8
1

Year Published

2010
2010
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 158 publications
(250 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
23
218
8
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Evidence of structural priming in both datives (Gries & Wulff, 2005;McDonough, 2006;Schoonbaert, Hartsuiker, & Pickering, 2007) and alternation in word order between adjective-noun phrases and relative clauses (Bernolet, Hartsuiker, & Pickering, 2007) are found in previous research. McDonough and Machey (2008) elaborate that structural priming can promote the production of the developmentally advanced language structure and suppress the production of the alternative structure in their interlanguage, which is considered to be a potential role of structural priming.…”
Section: Within-language Primingmentioning
confidence: 56%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Evidence of structural priming in both datives (Gries & Wulff, 2005;McDonough, 2006;Schoonbaert, Hartsuiker, & Pickering, 2007) and alternation in word order between adjective-noun phrases and relative clauses (Bernolet, Hartsuiker, & Pickering, 2007) are found in previous research. McDonough and Machey (2008) elaborate that structural priming can promote the production of the developmentally advanced language structure and suppress the production of the alternative structure in their interlanguage, which is considered to be a potential role of structural priming.…”
Section: Within-language Primingmentioning
confidence: 56%
“…Divergence of findings, however, exists in present studies. Bernolet, Hartsuiker, and Pickering (2007) found that cross-linguistic priming occurred only when the L1 and L2 had similar word order, that is, greater within-language priming occurred than between-language priming. Thereby, we can conclude that there is no complete sharing of syntactic representation between L1 and L2.…”
Section: Between-language Primingmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Some researches (Bernolet, Hartsuiker, & Pickering, 2007;Loebell & Bock, 2003) found that in the process of the cross-linguistic syntactic priming, the word sequence could be activated, while other researches (Chen, Jia, Wang, Dunlap, & Shin, 2013;Shin & Christianson, 2009; argued that the word sequence could not be activated.…”
Section: The Psychological Reality Of the Syntactic Hierarchical And mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…After the discovery of syntactic priming (also called structural persistence and structural priming) over 20 years ago, there have been numerous studies across a wide variety of populations. Syntactic priming has been the focus of studies with children (e.g., Garrod & Clark, 1993;Fisher, 2002;Tomasello, 2000), aphasiacs (e.g., Saffran & Martin, 1997), bilinguals (e.g., Bernolet, Hartsuiker, & Pickering, 2007;Schoonbaert, Hartsuiker, & Pickering, 2007), and second/foreign language learners (e.g., Gries & Wulff, 2005;Kim & McDonough, 2008;McDonough, 2006). Bock (1986) reported the first study which specifically used structural priming to investigate the processing and representation of language structures.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%