The platform will undergo maintenance on Sep 14 at about 7:45 AM EST and will be unavailable for approximately 2 hours.
2022
DOI: 10.1007/s10071-022-01653-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Serial visual reversal learning in harbor seals (Phoca vitulina)

Abstract: Progressively improving performance in a serial reversal learning (SRL) test has been associated with higher cognitive abilities and has served as a measure for cognitive/behavioral flexibility. Although the cognitive and sensory abilities of marine mammals have been subject of extensive investigation, and numerous vertebrate and invertebrate species were tested, SRL studies in aquatic mammals are sparse. Particularly in pinnipeds, a high degree of behavioral flexibility seems probable as they face a highly va… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 63 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Similarly, pigeons (Zeigler, 1961) reached around 60% of successful choices at second trial and around 70% on the last trial of the last few sets (80% when taking only the data from the most successful subjects), and although pigeons were exposed to many more sets of items than the rats, set numbers were still considerably less than in Harlow’s original study (with 120 sets presented versus 344 used in Harlow’s original experiment). After this original outburst of learning set experiments, later experiments focused on the “reversal” learning part of the paradigm, more as a measure of flexibility rather than of learning per se, often without being previously presented with a preceding sequential discrimination task, different to the original experiment (Bond et al, 2007; Erdsack et al, 2022; Rayburn-Reeves et al, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similarly, pigeons (Zeigler, 1961) reached around 60% of successful choices at second trial and around 70% on the last trial of the last few sets (80% when taking only the data from the most successful subjects), and although pigeons were exposed to many more sets of items than the rats, set numbers were still considerably less than in Harlow’s original study (with 120 sets presented versus 344 used in Harlow’s original experiment). After this original outburst of learning set experiments, later experiments focused on the “reversal” learning part of the paradigm, more as a measure of flexibility rather than of learning per se, often without being previously presented with a preceding sequential discrimination task, different to the original experiment (Bond et al, 2007; Erdsack et al, 2022; Rayburn-Reeves et al, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Two-alternative forced-choice tasks and go/no-go paradigms are common methodologies in human psychological and animal behaviour experiments. These tests require a participant to choose between two options; in the two-alternative forced-choice paradigm these options are two actual stimuli [ 1 , 2 ], while in the go/no-go paradigm the participant is expected to show a behaviour in response to a positive stimulus (“go”), and to inhibit that behaviour in response to a negative one (“no-go”) [ 3 , 4 ].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Let us consider a random sequence of two types of trials, A and B, in which the number of A/B trials is not balanced 50/50. In this case, an animal sticking to response A (e.g., because of a stimulus or side preference; [ 2 , 6 ]) will often be able to achieve >50% correct answers within an experimental session. Likewise, a random binary sequence might also feature many alternations of adjacent As and Bs.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation