2009
DOI: 10.3758/pbr.16.1.121
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sequential dependencies in the Eriksen flanker task: A direct comparison of two competing accounts

Abstract: The Eriksen flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974) has been frequently used to investigate attentional control processes. The basic task requires a fast response to a centrally presented target stimulus, which is flanked by several distractor stimuli that also activate response channels. A congruent trial is one in which the flankers are associated with the same response as the target (e.g., ), whereas in an incongruent trial, the flankers are associated with a competing response (e.g., ). Reaction times (RTs)… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
69
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 79 publications
(76 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
7
69
0
Order By: Relevance
“…According to domain-general theories of conflict control, conflict in one dimension (or task) can elicit control of conflict in an alternative dimension (or task, cf. Botvinick, Cohen, & Carter, 2004;Kunde & Wühr, 2006; but see, e.g., Davelaar & Stevens, 2009;Kiesel, Kunde & Hoffmann, 2006). Here, we tested whether conflict elicited in one domain by relations between the visible target's meaning and the responses affected the regulation of the conflict in an alternative domain, that is, between invisible prime and target.…”
Section: No Conflict Control In the Absence Of Awarenessmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…According to domain-general theories of conflict control, conflict in one dimension (or task) can elicit control of conflict in an alternative dimension (or task, cf. Botvinick, Cohen, & Carter, 2004;Kunde & Wühr, 2006; but see, e.g., Davelaar & Stevens, 2009;Kiesel, Kunde & Hoffmann, 2006). Here, we tested whether conflict elicited in one domain by relations between the visible target's meaning and the responses affected the regulation of the conflict in an alternative domain, that is, between invisible prime and target.…”
Section: No Conflict Control In the Absence Of Awarenessmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…An overview of the literature suggests that most Gratton experiments are confounded by either stimulus binding effects (e.g., Akçay & Hazeltine, 2007Davelaar & Stevens, 2009;Egner, Delano, & Hirsch, 2007;Funes, Lupiáñez, & Humphreys, 2010b;Kerns et al, 2004;Notebaert et al, 2006;Ullsperger, Bylsma, & Botvinick, 2005, Experiment 1;Wendt, Kluwe, & Peters, 2006) or contingency biases (e.g., Akçay & Hazeltine, 2007;Mayr & Awh, 2009;Ullsperger et al, 2005, Experiment 2;Verbruggen et al, 2006). Contingency biases are less common in the (typically two-choice) flanker tasks, but stimulus binding effects are much more difficult to control for in these two-choice flanker tasks, for reasons we will discuss later.…”
Section: The Binding Accountmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We do note, however, that 30% congruent items in a six-choice task is still well above chance (16.7%), thus not eliminating all contingency biases. Some studies do present congruent trials no more often than expected by chance, particularly in two-choice tasks (e.g., Davelaar & Stevens, 2009). However, these studies do not control for stimulus bindings (and cannot do so with a two-choice task).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Studies using various other response conflict paradigms found Gratton effects even when controlling for stimulus/response repetitions (e.g., Akcay & Hazeltine, 2007;Kerns et al, 2004;Kunde & Wühr, 2006;Ullsperger, Bylsma, & Botvinick, 2005). To accommodate these and other diverging results, more recent models of behavioral control and context adaptation favor a hybrid approach, combining associative bottom-up learning mechanisms with conflict-driven top-down control (e.g., Davelaar & Stevens, 2009;Verguts & Notebaert, 2009). An entirely different model has been proposed by Scherbaum, Dshemuchadse, Fischer, and Goschke (2010), who suggest that conflict adaptation reflects not a proactive mechanism, but the conflict-triggered reactivation of previouslyused conflict resolution processes.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%