2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.jet.2011.10.014
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Separating the Hawks from the Doves: Evidence from continuous time laboratory games

Abstract: Standard-Nutzungsbedingungen:Die Dokumente auf EconStor dürfen zu eigenen wissenschaftlichen Zwecken und zum Privatgebrauch gespeichert und kopiert werden.Sie dürfen die Dokumente nicht für öffentliche oder kommerzielle Zwecke vervielfältigen, öffentlich ausstellen, öffentlich zugänglich machen, vertreiben oder anderweitig nutzen.Sofern die Verfasser die Dokumente unter Open-Content-Lizenzen (insbesondere CC-Lizenzen) zur Verfügung gestellt haben sollten, gelten abweichend von diesen Nutzungsbedingungen die in… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

5
41
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 75 publications
(46 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
5
41
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We also see a sound separation of hawks and doves in our pure two-population (κ = 1) treatment. These findings confirm and extend the experiments of Oprea et al (2011). Among the discrepancies between the replicator dynamics prediction and the experimental results is a general bias in the mixed strategies: in the treatments where the mixed equilibrium was expected, the frequency of hawk play was lower than predicted.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 78%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We also see a sound separation of hawks and doves in our pure two-population (κ = 1) treatment. These findings confirm and extend the experiments of Oprea et al (2011). Among the discrepancies between the replicator dynamics prediction and the experimental results is a general bias in the mixed strategies: in the treatments where the mixed equilibrium was expected, the frequency of hawk play was lower than predicted.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 78%
“…Introduced by Maynard Smith and Price (1973) in the context of animal conflict, it also became highly influential for human interactions due to its fairly simple definition which nevertheless generates very rich dynamics as a population game. Oprea et al (2011) analyze the hawk-dove game for the sign-preserving dynamics of the one-and the two-population case. Their (continuous-time) experiment confirms the predictions in that the symmetric mixed equilibrium is more likely to be selected in the one-population treatment whereas separation is stronger in the two-population treatment.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The first one corresponds to continuous-time experiments, the second to "timing experiments", with a special emphasis on the experimental bank-runs literature. Continuous time experiments started years ago, with Friedman and Cheung (2009) and Morgan and Brunnermeier (2010) (whose working papers appeared around 2003/04), but it has not been until recently that this experimental technique has taken off with Oprea et al (2009) and Anderson et al (2010) looking into strategic investment decisions, Oprea et al (2011) studying the evolutionary equilibrium of the hawk and dove game, Friedman and Oprea (2012) experimenting with the effects of response delay in a repeated prisoners dilemma game, and Rabanal (2012) looking at mortgage default timing. While none of these papers directly address any of the questions of our paper, they are a good reference for the methodological design of our experiment.…”
Section: Why Run An Experiments On Abcp?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The games we study are networked generalizations of repeated or continuous versions of the game of Chicken or Hawk-Dove [9], 2-player instances and certain generalizations of which have been studied extensively in the lab [15,17,3]. The subject of fairness in human interactions has a very long history as well.…”
Section: Related Literaturementioning
confidence: 99%