2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.pragma.2011.10.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Semantic and pragmatic miscues in non-native spoken extended discourse

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Participants were asked to introduce themselves to the researcher and included information regarding the languages they spoke, their English language learning experience, which degree they were taking and why they had chosen to take it in English. This task was used since it has been found that learners are required to maintain longer stretches of speech, thus stimulating the need for increased discourse marking (Cribb 2012;González 2005). On the other hand, the interaction task (henceforth INT) required participants to engage in conversation with another participant.…”
Section: Instrumentsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Participants were asked to introduce themselves to the researcher and included information regarding the languages they spoke, their English language learning experience, which degree they were taking and why they had chosen to take it in English. This task was used since it has been found that learners are required to maintain longer stretches of speech, thus stimulating the need for increased discourse marking (Cribb 2012;González 2005). On the other hand, the interaction task (henceforth INT) required participants to engage in conversation with another participant.…”
Section: Instrumentsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Coherence can be glossed as how the units in a monologue 'hang together' (Dijk, 1997, 9 ). Cribb (2012) has operationalised this construct based on three criteria: specificity, consistency, and pragmatic relevance. In order to rate each participant for coherence, a rating exercise was undertaken by three raters independently of each other.…”
Section: Coherence Rating Exercisementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because pronunciation and prosody can have such an effect on perceptions of coherence, it was vital that these features were removed from the rating exercise. Thus a procedure initially adopted by Tyler & Bro (1992) and subsequently by Cribb (2012) was utilised in which each ASU in the monologue was presented to the rater in written form rather than aurally. While it is accepted that this is not how we normally judge coherence, in this case the procedure is necessary if we are to focus on the lexico-grammatical aspects of the monologue and not the phonological aspects.…”
Section: Coherence Rating Exercisementioning
confidence: 99%