2005
DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.11.009
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Self-motion perception from expanding and contracting optical flows overlapped with binocular disparity

Abstract: Expanding and contracting patterns were presented on different disparity planes to investigate the role of stereo depth in vection. Experiment 1 tested the effect of stereo depth on inducing vection with expanding and contracting flows on different disparity planes. Subjects reported whether they felt forward or backward self-motion. The results clearly showed the dominance of the background flow in determining one's self-motion direction. Experiment 2 tested the effect of stereo depth on a vection direction u… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
25
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
2
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Failures of imagination (rather than perception) could explain why it was not uncommon for subjects in such experiments to display left–right reversed heading judgments when they did not experience any vection during simulated translation (or display up–down reversed heading judgments during simulated landing – Palmisano and Gillam, 2005) 8 . Thus, Ito and Shibata (2005, p. 401) recently called for “detailed research comparing vection directions and perceived heading conducted with the same kind of stimuli.” In fact, there are already findings which suggest that performance on this sort of heading task does depend on whether the stimulus conditions are favorable for vection induction or not. For example, Grigo and Lappe (1998) reported that the accuracy of their subjects’ heading judgments changed markedly as the optic flow duration decreased from 3.2 s to only 0.4 s (Note: the latter duration would be too brief to induce any visual illusion of self-motion in a physically stationary observer).…”
Section: Challenge 2: Determining the Functional Significance Of Vectionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Failures of imagination (rather than perception) could explain why it was not uncommon for subjects in such experiments to display left–right reversed heading judgments when they did not experience any vection during simulated translation (or display up–down reversed heading judgments during simulated landing – Palmisano and Gillam, 2005) 8 . Thus, Ito and Shibata (2005, p. 401) recently called for “detailed research comparing vection directions and perceived heading conducted with the same kind of stimuli.” In fact, there are already findings which suggest that performance on this sort of heading task does depend on whether the stimulus conditions are favorable for vection induction or not. For example, Grigo and Lappe (1998) reported that the accuracy of their subjects’ heading judgments changed markedly as the optic flow duration decreased from 3.2 s to only 0.4 s (Note: the latter duration would be too brief to induce any visual illusion of self-motion in a physically stationary observer).…”
Section: Challenge 2: Determining the Functional Significance Of Vectionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The red target was perceived as nearer and also misperceived as smaller than the green target (Gentilucci, Benuzzi, Bartolani, & Gangitano, 2001). Depth is a mediating factor of vection strength, and the farthest stimulus dominates vection direction (e.g., Ito & Shibata, 2005). It is possible that red targets (red dots) were perceived as nearer than the green targets; thus, vection was attenuated more in red dot conditions than in green ones.…”
Section: On the Dot Color Effectmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This phenomenon is called vection (Fischer & Kornmuller, 1930). Several stimulus attributes are known to af-f f fect the subjective strength or direction of vection, such as depth (e.g., Ito & Shibata, 2005), visual area (e.g., Brandt, Dichgans, & Koenig, 1973), motion direction (e.g., Seno & Sato, 2009), and attention (e.g., Kitazaki & Sato, 2003). To date, color is one of the stimulus attributes in vection induction that has rarely been tested.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Already in 1975 demonstrated that the perceived self-rotation velocity (which is often used as a measure of the strength of circular vection) increases not only with the angular velocity of the visual stimulus as one might expect, but also linearly increases with the perceived distance of the moving stimulus. However, later research demonstrated that not only the absolute perceived distance, but in particular the relative depth structure and figure-ground (or object-background) separation seems critical, in that the stimulus that is perceived to be further away typically determines the occurrence, direction, and strength of vection Ito & Shibata, 2005;Nakamura, 2008;Nakamura & Shimojo, 1999;Ohmi & Howard, 1988;Ohmi et al, 1987). Several of these studies used perceptually bistable displays and demonstrated that not only physical stimulus parameters themselves, but in particular how the stimulus is perceived and interpreted at any moment in time can modulate or even determine self-motion perception.…”
Section: Increasing Retinal Slip Local Image Velocities and Relativmentioning
confidence: 99%