2014
DOI: 10.14417/lp.851
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Se correr o bicho pega... Normas de avaliação de imagens de animais negativos

Abstract: ResumoNeste trabalho apresentamos as normas de valência, familiaridade, medo, nojo (i.e., bee, gecko, centipede, mouse and cockroach) or non-domestic Laboratório de Psicologia, 12(1): 41-56 (2014) Abstract In this paper we present the norms for a set of 50 images of animals that were evaluated by a total sample of 78 college students regarding the following dimensions: valence, familiarity, fear, disgust and dangerousness. Ten categories of animals were selected based on the assumption of their negativ… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
(29 reference statements)
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The valence generated by an animal is likely to influence our emotional and behavioural responses toward them. For instance, animals such as spiders and snakes are generally negatively evaluated and often evoke an avoidance response [43,44]. In contrast, companion animals, such as dogs and cats, generally induce more positive evaluations and attachments [25,45,46].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The valence generated by an animal is likely to influence our emotional and behavioural responses toward them. For instance, animals such as spiders and snakes are generally negatively evaluated and often evoke an avoidance response [43,44]. In contrast, companion animals, such as dogs and cats, generally induce more positive evaluations and attachments [25,45,46].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The first stage of the systematic electronic search produced a combined result of 648 articles: 558 from the EBSCOhost database (334 from Academic Search Complete, 187 from PsycINFO, 25 from Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection and 12 from PsycARTICLES), 69 from Scopus and 21 from WoS. Four additional relevant studies were inserted on the data (i.e., Prada and Ricot, 2010;Prada et al, , 2014Brodeur et al, 2014) during the hand search phase. Despite not meeting all the inclusion criteria (some were not written in English) or not being retrieved in the systematic search (i.e., Brodeur et al, 2014), the inclusion of these articles was justified by their reporting of affective dimensions that were not explored in the elected papers (i.e., Prada and Ricot, 2010; valence and arousal examined in or because of the high number of images of common objects included (Brodeur et al, 2014).…”
Section: Retrieval Of the Studies And Literature Selectionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The remaining line-drawing studies used other stimuli created by the authors or selected from other sources (e.g., Ferraro et al, 1998;Duñabeitia et al, 2018). Among the photographs' studies sample, half used stimuli from the BOSS database (7; 50%) and the remaining used variations of common objects as stimuli embedded in contextual scenes (Shao and Stiegert, 2016), modified versions of object images (Prada and Ricot, 2010) and animals with negative valence (Prada et al, 2014). Moreover, two studies produced norms for both line-drawings and colored photographs (O'Sullivan et al, 2012;Saryazdi et al, 2018) comparing norms from Snodgrass and Vanderwart's (1980) and the BOSS (Brodeur et al, 2010(Brodeur et al, , 2014 databases.…”
Section: Stimuli Characteristicsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations