1995
DOI: 10.1080/03610739508254273
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Scope of Word Meaning Activation During Sentence Processing by Young and Older Adults

Abstract: In a naming experiment, we examined word meaning activation on-line during sentence processing by younger and older adults. Sentences were biased to either the most or least frequently used meaning of a sentence-final ambiguous word. In order to determine the scope of initial meaning activation, targets represented either high- or low-salient semantic relationships to a single sense of the ambiguous word in context. Both age groups evidenced context-dependent activation of word meaning. In addition, context ac… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

5
34
1

Year Published

2005
2005
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
5
34
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This has been demonstrated in semantic priming studies, in which a word or sentence prime that is semantically related to a following target word has been found to speed up recognition of the target (as indicated by correct lexical decision or naming) as much in older as in young adults (e.g., Burke, White, & Diaz, 1987;Madden, 1988Madden, , 1992Madden, Pierce, & Allen, 1993;Paul, 1996; for reviews, see Burke et al, 2000;Laver & Burke, 1993). Similarly, when ambiguous words are embedded in a sentence context (e.g., Some change was removed from her pockets), older adults are as efficient as young adults in using the semantic context to disambiguate the meaning of the words (e.g., Hopkins, Kellas, & Paul, 1995;Paul, 1996; but see Wingfield, Alexander, & Cavigelli, 1994).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This has been demonstrated in semantic priming studies, in which a word or sentence prime that is semantically related to a following target word has been found to speed up recognition of the target (as indicated by correct lexical decision or naming) as much in older as in young adults (e.g., Burke, White, & Diaz, 1987;Madden, 1988Madden, , 1992Madden, Pierce, & Allen, 1993;Paul, 1996; for reviews, see Burke et al, 2000;Laver & Burke, 1993). Similarly, when ambiguous words are embedded in a sentence context (e.g., Some change was removed from her pockets), older adults are as efficient as young adults in using the semantic context to disambiguate the meaning of the words (e.g., Hopkins, Kellas, & Paul, 1995;Paul, 1996; but see Wingfield, Alexander, & Cavigelli, 1994).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A few studies in the literature, however, have failed to find age differences in context use or other language com-prehension abilities (e.g., Burke & Harrold, 1988;Hopkins et al 1995;Light, Valencia-Laver, & Zavis, 1991). We have argued that context strength, time to process context, and processing efficiency or speed are all important factors determining whether a comprehender will make good use of contextual information during language comprehension.…”
Section: Context Time and Agingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some of these different outcomes may stem from variations in experimental procedures. For example, Hopkins, Kellas, and Paul (1995) reported good use of context by older adults in resolving lexical ambiguities during sentence comprehension, but these researchers used a method in which the visual presentation rate of words in the sentences was calibrated individually for each participant, with generally much slower rates for the older participants than for the younger ones. It is not hard to imagine that when processing efficiency differences are minimized by changes in presentation rates, older and younger adults could show highly similar performance, whereas clear differences could emerge when both groups are tested with identical presentation conditions.…”
Section: Ambiguities and Cognitive Agingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This pattern is consistent with Dagerman et al's (2006) observation of less effective on-line ambiguity resolution in older adults when semantic contextual support was not available. More generally, the pattern of aging effects we observe across the two context types provides a reconciliation of the disparate patterns that have been observed across the literature looking at age effects on ambiguity resolution (Balota & Duchek, 1991;Dagerman, MacDonald, & Harm, 2006;Hopkins, Kellas, & Paul, 1995;Meyer & Federmeier, 2010;Swaab, Brown, & Hagoort, 1998), in showing that when age-related differences will be observed depends on the nature of the context information available to support ambiguity resolution.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 78%
“…However, perhaps surprisingly, empirical work looking at the effects of aging on lexical ambiguity resolution has yielded mixed results. On the one hand, several studies, including Hopkins, Kellas, & Paul (1995), Swaab, Brown, & Hagoort (1998, Balota &Duchek (1991), andMeyer &Federmeier (2010) older adults did not demonstrate the kind of rapid, online context use in the service of ambiguity resolution that was seen for younger adults, even though both groups were identical in their context use in off-line sentence completions and compatibility judgment tasks.Apart from methodological differences, including type of measure (behavioral or electrophysiological) and task (naming vs. passive reading), one critical difference between these studies is in the nature and availability of the disambiguating information provided by the experimental materials. Specifically, all four studies showing young-like patterns in healthy older adults used contexts that provided coherent lexical-semantic constraints.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%