2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.04.018
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reward and Affective Regulation in Depression-Prone Smokers

Abstract: Background There is a disproportionately high smoking prevalence among individuals who are prone to depression. While depression has been conceptualized as a disorder of dysregulated positive affect and disrupted reward processing, little research has been conducted to determine the role of smoking in these processes among depression-prone smokers. Methods Depression-prone smokers (DP+; n = 34) and smokers not depression-prone (DP-; n=49) underwent two laboratory sessions, once while smoking abstinent and on… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

5
39
2

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 51 publications
(46 citation statements)
references
References 109 publications
5
39
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Specifically, after overnight abstinence from smoking, the DP+ group responded more slowly to positive compared with neutral stimuli, which is thought to reflect the allocation of resources to emotionally salient stimuli (Epp et al, 2012). These data are also consistent with those from the larger study that found that smoking abstinence increased negative affect across all smokers, but the DP+ group also showed greater changes in positive affect (Audrain-McGovern et al, in press). In addition, depression-prone smokers experienced a greater loss of pleasure from other reinforcers during nicotine withdrawal suggesting that smoking may regulate positive affect, rather than simply reducing negative affect for depression-prone smokers (Audrain-McGovern et al, in press).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Specifically, after overnight abstinence from smoking, the DP+ group responded more slowly to positive compared with neutral stimuli, which is thought to reflect the allocation of resources to emotionally salient stimuli (Epp et al, 2012). These data are also consistent with those from the larger study that found that smoking abstinence increased negative affect across all smokers, but the DP+ group also showed greater changes in positive affect (Audrain-McGovern et al, in press). In addition, depression-prone smokers experienced a greater loss of pleasure from other reinforcers during nicotine withdrawal suggesting that smoking may regulate positive affect, rather than simply reducing negative affect for depression-prone smokers (Audrain-McGovern et al, in press).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…These data are also consistent with those from the larger study that found that smoking abstinence increased negative affect across all smokers, but the DP+ group also showed greater changes in positive affect (Audrain-McGovern et al, in press). In addition, depression-prone smokers experienced a greater loss of pleasure from other reinforcers during nicotine withdrawal suggesting that smoking may regulate positive affect, rather than simply reducing negative affect for depression-prone smokers (Audrain-McGovern et al, in press). Perhaps the reduction in the rewarding effects of other reinforcers during nicotine withdrawal contributed to the abstinence-induced attentional bias to positive stimuli in the DP+ group.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Compared to adults who smoke and do not have depression, adults who smoke and have depression more strongly endorse beliefs about smoking including smoking to relieve negative affect (71,72). Adult smokers with depression also respond more strongly to smoking cues (73,74) and report greater reward from smoking (75)(76)(77). Further, adult smokers with depression also report more withdrawal symptoms and more severe symptoms of withdrawal during quit attempts (73,(78)(79)(80).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…The maximal effort exerted before choosing not to respond further provides a measure of motivation, referred to as a “breakpoint.” PRTs have been used to study motivation in humans, primarily in the context of addiction. For example, depressed smokers show greater PRT motivation to obtain nicotine than money (Audrain-McGovern et al, 2014) and recreational drinkers show increased PRT motivation for alcohol following depressed mood induction (Willner et al, 1998b). A strength of the PRT is its applicability in both animal models and humans, making this task especially useful for translational investigation (Scheggi et al, 2015; Willner et al, 1998a).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%