2015
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0139797
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reversing Stimulus Timing in Visual Conditioning Leads to Memories with Opposite Valence in Drosophila

Abstract: Animals need to associate different environmental stimuli with each other regardless of whether they temporally overlap or not. Drosophila melanogaster displays olfactory trace conditioning, where an odor is followed by electric shock reinforcement after a temporal gap, leading to conditioned odor avoidance. Reversing the stimulus timing in olfactory conditioning results in the reversal of memory valence such that an odor that follows shock is later on approached (i.e. relief conditioning). Here, we explored t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
22
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
(66 reference statements)
3
22
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Behavioral responses shaped by relief learning appear to be of weaker strength (Tanimoto et al, 2004; Gerber et al, 2014; Vogt et al, 2015), which corresponds well with our own findings. However, it is important to note that our paradigm differed from the previous studies on relief-learning in Drosophila , most notably in that it contained an operant element (shocks could be avoided).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Behavioral responses shaped by relief learning appear to be of weaker strength (Tanimoto et al, 2004; Gerber et al, 2014; Vogt et al, 2015), which corresponds well with our own findings. However, it is important to note that our paradigm differed from the previous studies on relief-learning in Drosophila , most notably in that it contained an operant element (shocks could be avoided).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…In this work, this definition will be used to refer to an appetitive experience. These kind of aversive and relief memories could be found in phylogenetically distant species such as fly, rats, mice and humans (Andreatta et al, 2012; Vogt et al, 2015). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Punishment-memory scores were dramatically stronger than relief-memory scores, as was the case in all previous datasets directly comparing these two kinds of memory in the fruit fly [28] (see [12] for a meta-analysis). Indeed, among the 38 inbred strains tested, all showed significant punishment-memory, except one strain with a tendency only for punishment-memory; whereas six strains had significant relief-memory, with nine further strains showing a tendency towards it (figure 2 a , one-sided one-sample Wilcoxon signed-rank tests using a false discovery rate less than 0.05 for significance criterion and p < 0.05 for tendency criterion; see electronic supplementary material, table S1 for data and statistical report).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 76%