1988
DOI: 10.3758/bf03209037
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Response sequence learning as a function of primary versus conditioned reinforcement

Abstract: Two groups of pigeons were required to generate a fixed sequence of responses on three keys, for example, middle-left-right. One group received a small food reward (SFood) following each correct response except the terminal one, which was followed by a large food reward. The second group received conditioned reinforcement from an overhead light (SLight) for each correct response, with the terminal correct response followed by both SLight and the large food reward. We manipulated length of sequence (3 or 7 resp… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

1989
1989
1994
1994

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In the present Phase 1, when reinforcement was made contingent on one of a number of possible response sequences, the frequency of emission of the reinforced sequence increased, and exceeded the frequency of emission of all other possible sequences. Although this effect has been demonstrated extensively in pigeons (Fetterman & Stubbs, 1982;Grayson & Wasserman, 1979;Luck et al, 1988;Stubbs et al, 1987;Wasserman et al, 1984), few studies have demonstrated this effect in rats (but see Schachtman & Reed, in press). The present study also demonstrated that presenting a signal along with reinforcement facilitated the acquisition of the required response sequence (see also Reed et al, in press).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In the present Phase 1, when reinforcement was made contingent on one of a number of possible response sequences, the frequency of emission of the reinforced sequence increased, and exceeded the frequency of emission of all other possible sequences. Although this effect has been demonstrated extensively in pigeons (Fetterman & Stubbs, 1982;Grayson & Wasserman, 1979;Luck et al, 1988;Stubbs et al, 1987;Wasserman et al, 1984), few studies have demonstrated this effect in rats (but see Schachtman & Reed, in press). The present study also demonstrated that presenting a signal along with reinforcement facilitated the acquisition of the required response sequence (see also Reed et al, in press).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…The acquisition of response sequences has been the focus of a number of investigations (e.g., Arbuckle & Lattal, 1987;Grayson & Wasserman, 1979;Hawkes & Shimp, 1975;Luck, Colgrove, & Neuringer, 1988;Schwartz, 1980Schwartz, , 1982Vogel & Annau, 1973;Wasserman et al, 1984). For example, Grayson and Wasserman (1979; see also Catania, 1971;Silberberg & Williams, 1974) illuminated two response keys and required pigeons to perform a two-response sequence (left-left, leftright, right-left, or right-right) to obtain reinforcement.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Luck, Colgrove, and Neuringer (1988) trained pigeons to generate fixed sequences ofresponse on three keys (e.g., middle-left-right). One group received a 2-s food reward after each correct response except the terminal response, which produced a 6-s food reward.…”
Section: Practical Uses Of Conditioned Reinforcementmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In any event, one conclusion follows: In comparison to Stubbs et al's (1987) demonstration of sequences as units, delay effects here were presumably strong enough to prevent similar results. In other experimental contexts, reinforcement rate has also been an important variable (e.g., Luck, Colgrove, & Neuringer, 1988), but at the reinforcement rate ranges explored in Experiment 1 no comparable effects were noted.…”
Section: Behavioral Unitsmentioning
confidence: 86%