1968
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1968.11-503
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

RESPONSE RATE, REINFORCEMENT FREQUENCY, AND CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION1

Abstract: In the first of two experiments, periods of noise were terminated with unavoidable shock to 36 rats. The rats' continuously reinforced responding was later completely suppressed during the noise when it was introduced without shock. The rats were then assigned to nine experimental groups. Each group was exposed to different paced variable-interval schedules of reinforcement, which independently controlled response rate and reinforcement frequency. Periods of the noise were periodically superimposed on these sc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

7
54
2
9

Year Published

1972
1972
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
2
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 116 publications
(87 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
(24 reference statements)
7
54
2
9
Order By: Relevance
“…As noted above, with reinforcement rate equated, it has been shown several times that lower response rates are more resistant than higher response rates (see also Blackman, 1968;Lattal, Reilly, & Kohn, 1998; but see Fath, Fields, Malott, & Grossett, 1983). Accounts offered for this greater resistance of lower response rates have included, among others, the aversiveness of high response rates (Lattal et al), response elasticity (Nevin et aI., 2001), and operant response class size (Doughty & Lattal, 2001;Reed & Doughty, 2005).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…As noted above, with reinforcement rate equated, it has been shown several times that lower response rates are more resistant than higher response rates (see also Blackman, 1968;Lattal, Reilly, & Kohn, 1998; but see Fath, Fields, Malott, & Grossett, 1983). Accounts offered for this greater resistance of lower response rates have included, among others, the aversiveness of high response rates (Lattal et al), response elasticity (Nevin et aI., 2001), and operant response class size (Doughty & Lattal, 2001;Reed & Doughty, 2005).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Different schedules of reinforcement promote markedly different patterns of responding in nonhumans (e.g., Ferster & Skinner, 1957). In addition, schedules have been noted to modulate basic behavioral processes such as positive and negative conditioned suppression (Blackman, 1968), reinforcer devaluation (Adams, 1982), and signaled reward effects (Reed, Schachtman, & Hall, 1988). Given the seemingly ubiquitous influence of schedules of reinforcement on behavior, coupled with the recent interest in the influence of the contingency between response and outcome on human judgments of causation, it would appear sensible to examine the role of schedules relating responses to outcomes in human judgments of causal effectiveness.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Males also respond more often when food presentations are responsedependent, both in procedures that selectively reinforce low response rates (Millar, 1975;van Hest et al, 1987a) and in procedures in which high response rates are required for food to be presented (Heinsbroek, van Haaren, Zantvoord, & van de Poll, 1987;van Haaren, Heinsbroek, Louwerse, & van de Poll, 1986). Available data suggest that high response rates are more resistant to change than lower response rates (Blackman, 1968;Nevin, 1974), which might explain the less efficient discriminationreversal performance of males in free-operant conditioning procedures. Other experiments, however, will have to be designed to investigate this notion explicitly.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%