2021
DOI: 10.1177/23969415211053264
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Relations between language, non-verbal cognition, and conceptualization in non- or minimally verbal individuals with ASD across the lifespan

Abstract: Background & aims Individuals with non- or minimally verbal autism (nvASD) are primarily characterized by a severe speech production deficit, with speech limited to no or only a few words by school age. Significant unclarity remains over variability in language profiles across the lifespan, the nature of the language impairment seen, and (dis-) associations between linguistic and nonverbal cognitive measures. Methods To address these questions, we recruited both a school-age and an adult group with nvASD (… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
5

Citation Types

1
18
0
1

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

4
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
1
18
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…To the same extent, aspects of aphasic speech said to be ‘agrammatic’ may be limited as a way of illuminating the neural basis of grammar and yield a distorted picture of the grammatical competence involved. To understand the neural basis of a true loss of such competence, other disease models would need to be considered, such as those 30% of children and adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) who never develop functional language to begin with, and who remain restricted in their linguistic repertoire to no or a handful of single words, with a uniform absence of grammar in production and comprehension [ 23 , 24 ]. On the other hand, the hypothesis of intactness is by no means uncontroversial and an equally prominent foundational view defines aphasia as language impairment dissociated from cognitive impairment [ 25 ].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To the same extent, aspects of aphasic speech said to be ‘agrammatic’ may be limited as a way of illuminating the neural basis of grammar and yield a distorted picture of the grammatical competence involved. To understand the neural basis of a true loss of such competence, other disease models would need to be considered, such as those 30% of children and adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) who never develop functional language to begin with, and who remain restricted in their linguistic repertoire to no or a handful of single words, with a uniform absence of grammar in production and comprehension [ 23 , 24 ]. On the other hand, the hypothesis of intactness is by no means uncontroversial and an equally prominent foundational view defines aphasia as language impairment dissociated from cognitive impairment [ 25 ].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Many studies simply describe affected children as having “intellectual disability” or “speech delay”, even at times seeming to use these terms interchangeably (e.g., [ 19 , 83 ]). Others employ more specific measures of IQ (intelligence quotient), language, and speech to produce more detailed and accurate phenotypes (e.g., [ 82 ]). But because IQ, language, and speech are separable (though interacting) cognitive domains with distinct (though overlapping) neural substrates, in order to understand the full range of effects of specific genetic mutations, it is necessary to evaluate them all using psychometrically sound measures.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…(2019) , however, a potentially more powerful source of insight comes from school-age children with neurodevelopmental disorders, who do not develop language in either production or comprehension at all. This includes 30% of people on the autism spectrum ( Norrelgen et al., 2015 ; Slusna et al., 2021 ) – a population that, while neglected in research terms ( Jack and Pelphrey, 2017 ), is of considerable importance for raising fundamental questions about the neural basis of language and cognitive functioning under conditions of language absence ( Hinzen et al., 2019 ). Such children are unlikely to develop language later ( Slusna et al., 2021 ) and provide us with an alternative and arguably closer approximation of a ‘language-less’ mind than either neurotypical infants or verbally shadowing adults could do.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This includes 30% of people on the autism spectrum ( Norrelgen et al., 2015 ; Slusna et al., 2021 ) – a population that, while neglected in research terms ( Jack and Pelphrey, 2017 ), is of considerable importance for raising fundamental questions about the neural basis of language and cognitive functioning under conditions of language absence ( Hinzen et al., 2019 ). Such children are unlikely to develop language later ( Slusna et al., 2021 ) and provide us with an alternative and arguably closer approximation of a ‘language-less’ mind than either neurotypical infants or verbally shadowing adults could do. Absence or regression of language development is by no means confined to non- or minimally verbal autism, moreover, but found in various instantiations and degrees across numerous neurogenetic conditions including Phelan-McDermid, Angelman, Coffin-Siris, Landau-Kleffner, Rett, and Cri du Chat syndromes ( Hinzen et al., 2019 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%