1994
DOI: 10.1098/rspb.1994.0103
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Relational colour constancy from invariant cone-excitation ratios

Abstract: Quantitative measurements of perceptual colour constancy show that human observers have a limited and variable ability to match coloured surfaces in scenes illuminated by different light sources. Observers can, however, make fast and reliable discriminations between changes in illuminant and changes in the reflecting properties of scenes, a discriminative ability that might be based on a visual coding of spatial colour relations. This coding could be provided by the ratios of cone-photoreceptor excitations pro… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

4
74
0

Year Published

2000
2000
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 232 publications
(78 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
4
74
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These results, using stimuli with gray backgrounds, confirm results using stimuli with different colored backgrounds (Conway, 2001). Such "doubleopponent" receptive fields are ideal substrates for color constancy and color contrast (Hurlbert and Poggio, 1988;Dufort and Lumsden, 1991;Foster and Nascimento, 1994;Gegenfurtner, 2003;Hurlbert and Wolf, 2004), computations that likely involve V1 (Barbur et al, 2004) (but see Zeki et al, 1999). Some doubleopponent receptive fields were circularly symmetric, with round centers and doughnut-shaped surrounds, but most had round centers with crescent-shaped surrounds, or two parallel ovalshaped subfields.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 74%
“…These results, using stimuli with gray backgrounds, confirm results using stimuli with different colored backgrounds (Conway, 2001). Such "doubleopponent" receptive fields are ideal substrates for color constancy and color contrast (Hurlbert and Poggio, 1988;Dufort and Lumsden, 1991;Foster and Nascimento, 1994;Gegenfurtner, 2003;Hurlbert and Wolf, 2004), computations that likely involve V1 (Barbur et al, 2004) (but see Zeki et al, 1999). Some doubleopponent receptive fields were circularly symmetric, with round centers and doughnut-shaped surrounds, but most had round centers with crescent-shaped surrounds, or two parallel ovalshaped subfields.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 74%
“…Some of this comes under the rubric of color appearancẽ Wyszecki, 1986;Shevell, 2003! and of color order systems Derefeldt, 1991;Brainard, 2003!. A second literature emphasizes the effects of changing illumination~Helson, 1940; Burnham et al, 1957;McCann, 1976;Arend & Reeves, 1986;Brainard & Wandell, 1992;Foster & Nascimento, 1994;Brainard, 1998;Bauml, 1999;Delahunt & Brainard, 2004;Hansen et al, 2007! or effects of scene geometry on the appearance of flat test surfaces~Bloj et al, 1999;Yang & Maloney, 2001;Khang & Zaidi, 2002;Boyaci et al, 2003Boyaci et al, , 2004Ripamonti et al, 2004!.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The ability of subjects to make accurate relational judgments independent of the illuminant, as with these minimalist scenes, has been attributed to 'relational colour constancy' [28]; that is, the constancy of perceived colour relations under different illuminants. It was initially conceived for situations in which scenes differ only in illuminant, so that one surface could be related to another -the simplest possible comparison.…”
Section: Relational Colour Constancymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Relational colour constancy can be interpreted operationally: subjects are simply asked to discriminate between illuminant and surface-reflectance changes [28,29]. Performance in one such task has been found to be fast, accurate and effortless, suggesting that some aspects of surface-colour information are processed in parallel over the visual field [27].…”
Section: Relational Colour Constancymentioning
confidence: 99%