2018
DOI: 10.1101/366393
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reference Repulsion Is Not a Perceptual Illusion

Abstract: Perceptual decisions are often influenced by contextual factors. For instance, when engaged in a visual discrimination task against a reference boundary, subjective reports about the judged stimulus feature are biased away from the boundary -a phenomenon termed reference repulsion. Until recently, this phenomenon has been thought to reflect a perceptual illusion regarding the appearance of the stimulus, but new evidence suggests that it may rather reflect a post-perceptual decision bias. To shed light on this … Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
2
1

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In those experiments, choices for the current stimulus are typically distorted towards the preceding stimulus [ 68 , 69 ], an attractive effect that is thought to underlie stable perception. However, the sign of this effect flips depending on the nature of the task [ 70 , 71 ], with tasks that require a comparison between stimuli and a standard, as our task does, documenting repulsion.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In those experiments, choices for the current stimulus are typically distorted towards the preceding stimulus [ 68 , 69 ], an attractive effect that is thought to underlie stable perception. However, the sign of this effect flips depending on the nature of the task [ 70 , 71 ], with tasks that require a comparison between stimuli and a standard, as our task does, documenting repulsion.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Since first proposed (Stocker and Simoncelli, 2007), data from a growing number of psychophysical studies suggest that humans perform conditioned inference in certain situations (Jazayeri and Movshon, 2007; Zamboni et al, 2016; Wu et al, 2009; Ding et al, 2017; Fritsche and de Lange, 2019; Luu and Stocker, 2018); i.e. , they commit to a single high-level interpretation rather than taking into account all possible high-level interpretations when performing low-level feature inference.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, by making an category assignment a subject’s subsequent perceptual estimate of a low-level stimulus feature ( e.g. , motion direction (Jazayeri and Movshon, 2007; Zamboni et al, 2016) or visual orientation (Luu and Stocker, 2018; Fritsche and de Lange, 2019)) is biased towards the assigned category on a per trial basis. These biases can be thought of as a form of consistency (Brehm, 1956) or confirmation bias (Nickerson, 1998) where the perceptual estimate aligns with and confirms the chosen category (Bronfman et al, 2015; Talluri et al, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, perception is not only biased by objective, statistical prior expectations but also by an observer's subjective categorical assessment of a visual feature. For example, in a task sequence where subjects first had to make a categorical judgment about a stimulus feature before providing an estimate of the feature value (motion direction 7,8,9 ; orientation 10,11 ; or numerosity 9,12 ), the resulting estimates showed systematic biases in favor of their preceding category choices.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%