2019
DOI: 10.1101/658914
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Benefits of commitment in hierarchical inference

Abstract: Humans have the tendency to commit to a single interpretation of what has caused some observed 1 evidence rather than considering all possible alternatives. This tendency can explain various forms 2 of confirmation and reference biases. However, committing to a single high-level interpretation 3 seems short-sighted and irrational, and thus it is unclear why humans seem motivated to pursue 4 such strategy. 5 In a first step toward answering this question, we systematically quantified how this strategy affect… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Therefore, the belief state partly discarded decision-incongruent information in favor of self-consistency across the task stages (Peters et al, 2017). As argued in both Peters et al (2017) and Luu and Stocker (2018), these seemingly sub-optimal behaviors might be resource-rational in natural settings with many hidden states and under limited cognitive capacities (see also Qiu et al (2020)).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Therefore, the belief state partly discarded decision-incongruent information in favor of self-consistency across the task stages (Peters et al, 2017). As argued in both Peters et al (2017) and Luu and Stocker (2018), these seemingly sub-optimal behaviors might be resource-rational in natural settings with many hidden states and under limited cognitive capacities (see also Qiu et al (2020)).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…In the current cross-situational learning task, where each trial provides new evidence that may help to refine the word-meaning mapping, a bias to persist in one's own belief and to discard new information may seem to be maladaptive. However, in real word learning scenarios, when one's own lexical knowledge is thought to be reliable, it may be rational to ignore conflicting information, presumably outliers or uninformative observations, to increase the robustness of the learning process, especially as the hypothesis space of possible meanings is vast and not explicit (see relatedly Oaksford & Chater, 1994;Qiu, Luu, & Stocker, 2020;Tsetsos et al, 2016). Perhaps most obviously, it is hard to see how inferential processes such as mutual exclusivity would work without assessing the reliability of one's knowledge about word meanings: either one would apply mutual exclusivity immediately, using words for which one does not have much evidence to learn the meaning of other words (which would lead to a cascade of errors), or one would never apply it because it is objectively impossible to eliminate all uncertainty.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Zamboni, Ledgeway, McGraw, & Schluppeck, 2016). Such a scheme is suboptimal (Fleming, Maloney, & Daw, 2013), although it can be beneficial in the presence of later distortions of the stimulus representation (Qiu, Luu, and Stocker 2020;and closely related Li, Castañón, Solomon, Vandormael, and Summerfield 2017). Also in contrast to our point estimate observers, the original categorisation decision is based on full Bayesian inference.…”
Section: Bayesian Observermentioning
confidence: 97%