2010
DOI: 10.3758/pbr.17.6.923
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reconsidering “evidence” for fast-and-frugal heuristics

Abstract: In several recent reviews, authors have argued for the pervasive use of fast-and-frugal

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

1
55
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 64 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 73 publications
1
55
0
Order By: Relevance
“…An important point of contention is that toolbox models as a whole are difficult to falsify and it is not always clear how they can be tested against alternative models of cognition (e.g., Bröder, 2000;Dougherty, Franco-Watkins, & Thomas, 2008;Hilbig, 2010;Newell, 2005;Todd & Gigerenzer, 2001).…”
Section: Difficulties When Testing Toolbox Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An important point of contention is that toolbox models as a whole are difficult to falsify and it is not always clear how they can be tested against alternative models of cognition (e.g., Bröder, 2000;Dougherty, Franco-Watkins, & Thomas, 2008;Hilbig, 2010;Newell, 2005;Todd & Gigerenzer, 2001).…”
Section: Difficulties When Testing Toolbox Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Over the past decade, the RH has attracted significant attention and has generated much controversy (Hilbig, 2010;Marewski, Pohl, & Vitouch, 2010, 2011a, 2011bPachur, Bröder, & Marewski, 2008;Pachur et al, 2011). From a processing perspective, three issues have been particularly pertinent.…”
Section: Ffh Framework: the Recognition Heuristic (Rh)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…From a processing perspective, three issues have been particularly pertinent. First, the idea that any knowledge beyond recognition is ignored (at the PMM stage) has been very controversial from the outset, with one camp claiming to provide evidence in support of noncompensatory processing (e.g., Goldstein & Gigerenzer, 2002;Marewski, Gaissmaier, et al, 2010;Pachur & Hertwig, 2006;Volz et al, 2006) and an opposing camp claiming to provide evidence against it (e.g., Bröder & Eichler, 2006;Glöckner & Bröder, 2011;Hilbig, 2010;Hilbig, Erdfelder, & Pohl, 2010;Hilbig, Pohl, & Bröder, 2009;Newell & Fernandez, 2006;Newell & Shanks, 2004;Oppenheimer, 2003;Pohl, 2006;Richter & Späth, 2006). Second, the RH is assumed to operate on the strictly binary output of a recognition judgment; however, the link between the recognition-based inference and the underlying recognition process has remained unclear (e.g., Dougherty, Franco-Watkins, & Thomas, 2008;Erdfelder, Küpper-Tetzel, & Mattern, 2011;Pleskac, 2007;Rosburg, Mecklinger, & Frings, 2011;Schooler & Hertwig, 2005).…”
Section: Ffh Framework: the Recognition Heuristic (Rh)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Table 1), it is ecologically rational to rely on the recognition heuristic . Recognition validity a a = R/(R + W), "where R is the number of correct (right) inferences the recognition heuristic would achieve, computed across all pairs in which one object is recognized and the other is not, and W is the number of incorrect (wrong) inferences under the same circumstances" (Goldstein & Gigerenzer, 2002, p. 78 Over the last 10 years of research on the recognition heuristic, evidence has been provided that people indeed adopt this heuristic in environments in which it yields accurate inferences (for a review, see ; for an overview of current studies, see special issues edited by Marewski, Pohl, & Vitouch, 2010, 2011a; for a critical position, see Hilbig, 2010;and Pohl, 2011), namely, people appear to select their strategies in an adaptive way by relying on recognition information when recognition is systematically related to a criterion but discounting it when it is not related (e.g., Pachur & Hertwig, 2006;Pohl, 2006). Hence, there is a positive correlation of r = .64 between the recognition validity and the proportion of judgments consistent with the recognition heuristic across 11 studies (Pachur, Todd, Gigerenzer, Schooler, & Goldstein, 2011).…”
Section: The Recognition Heuristic In Individualsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although we cannot expect that verbalized reasoning exactly mirrors the strategies actually used (as those might not be accessible to people; e.g., Nisbett & Wilson, 1977), the process may still reveal participants' subjective, important reasons and provide a source for new insights for researchers (see Keller, Gummerum, Canz, Gigerenzer, & Takezawa, 2013; for a similar claim). This may help to address the question of whether people really use the recognition heuristic (Hilbig, 2010), that is, whether they rely on their recognition when the recognition heuristic models their choices best. Translated to the group level, the question is whether the recognition cue is a behavioral correlate of the RBM and thus more often used during discussion by groups that predominantly use the RBM than by groups that predominantly use the KBM.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%