PsycEXTRA Dataset 1992
DOI: 10.1037/e665412011-176
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reasoning About Curvilinear Motion: Using Principles or Analogy

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

1
2
0

Year Published

2001
2001
2002
2002

Publication Types

Select...
2

Relationship

0
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
1
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The results of some studies suggest observers may be sensitive to or directly perceive effects of causal dynamics (e.g., Bingham, 1987;Runeson & Frykholm, 1983;Valenti & Costall, 1997). However, the results of other studies suggest observers may appeal to heuristics instead of directly perceiving causal dynamics (e.g., , and these latter findings are more consistent with post-Humean notions that causality must be inferred from contiguity and with findings in so-called naïve physics (Catrambone, Jones, Jonides, & Seifert, 1995;Cooke & Breedin, 1994). In a series of landmark studies on the perception of mechanical causality, Michotte (1946Michotte ( /1963see also Thinès, Costall, & Butterworth, 1991) demonstrated that observers made causal attributions when they viewed various types of interacting stimuli.…”
supporting
confidence: 52%
“…The results of some studies suggest observers may be sensitive to or directly perceive effects of causal dynamics (e.g., Bingham, 1987;Runeson & Frykholm, 1983;Valenti & Costall, 1997). However, the results of other studies suggest observers may appeal to heuristics instead of directly perceiving causal dynamics (e.g., , and these latter findings are more consistent with post-Humean notions that causality must be inferred from contiguity and with findings in so-called naïve physics (Catrambone, Jones, Jonides, & Seifert, 1995;Cooke & Breedin, 1994). In a series of landmark studies on the perception of mechanical causality, Michotte (1946Michotte ( /1963see also Thinès, Costall, & Butterworth, 1991) demonstrated that observers made causal attributions when they viewed various types of interacting stimuli.…”
supporting
confidence: 52%
“…However, even when these examples are potentially useful, students may not be able to make use of the analogy between a new problem and the example if they are hindered by a superficial distinction (Lancaster & Kolodner, 1987;Novick, 1988;Ross, 1989). According to Catrambone, Jones, Jonides, and Seifert (1995), when solving a problem that reminds them of a familiar example, people often reason by analogy to the instance. However, as noted earlier, instances are often retrieved on the basis of superficial similarity (Gentner, Rattermann, & Forbus, 1993;Holyoak & Koh, 1987;Ross, 1989).…”
Section: Implications For Educationmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…For example, in our study, the unusual situation of a coffee cup with an ice cube in it was described by participants as a scientific experiment, whereas the familiar situation of cooking pancakes was understood as a real-world example. From this viewpoint, people may be unaware that seemingly distant situations have matching underlying structures (Catrambone et al, 1995;Kaiser, Jonides, & Alexander, 1986). It can be hard to help students comprehend the general principles underlying both a science experiment and everyday life experiences given the variation in context and surface details.…”
Section: Implications For Educationmentioning
confidence: 98%