2015
DOI: 10.11114/smc.v3i2.1022
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reactance and Public Health Messages: The Unintended Dangers of Anti-tobacco PSAs

Abstract: This study examined smokers' reactions to antismoking messages that emphasized either the harms of secondhand smoke or vulnerability to smoking addiction and framed the need for smoking cessation using either gain or loss message frames. One hundred fifty-four college smokers participated in a study that used a 2 x 2 factorial design crossing message type (secondhand smoke appeals, smoking addiction appeals) and message frame (gain, loss) with the addition of a control group. The primary outcomes assessed were… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Message evaluation. Some studies employ broad "cognitive evaluation" or "message evaluation" scales that are positively or neutrally valenced, with lower scores used to represent cognitive reactance (e.g., Wong et al, 2015). These are based on approaches from Grandpre et al (2003), who used a semantic differential scale assessing whether participants found a message "good/bad," "valuable/worthless," and so forth, and Miller et al (2007), who used a cognitive evaluation scale to assess whether respondents found a message to be fair, interesting, and relevant to them.…”
Section: Message Minimizationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Message evaluation. Some studies employ broad "cognitive evaluation" or "message evaluation" scales that are positively or neutrally valenced, with lower scores used to represent cognitive reactance (e.g., Wong et al, 2015). These are based on approaches from Grandpre et al (2003), who used a semantic differential scale assessing whether participants found a message "good/bad," "valuable/worthless," and so forth, and Miller et al (2007), who used a cognitive evaluation scale to assess whether respondents found a message to be fair, interesting, and relevant to them.…”
Section: Message Minimizationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such scales often capture both global criticism and refutation of specific points. For example, variations of the Silvia scale asked “Did you criticize the message you just saw while you were reading it?” ( global ; Gardner & Leshner, 2016, p. 743) and “I came up with specific responses to the arguments made in the message” ( specific ; Wong, Harrison, & Harvell, 2015, p. 77). Niederdeppe et al (2011) used thought listing to capture counterarguing, but took a unique approach: Rather than treat counterarguing as a solely cognitive response, they distinguished between cognitively and emotionally reactive counterarguments.…”
Section: Capturing Reactance In Communicationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Past research showed that exposure to messages the attempt to persuade individuals to refrain from risky behaviors, such as smoking, marijuana use, and consuming sugarsweetened beverages activated psychological reactance, especially when the PSAs included a fear appeal [56][57][58][59][60]. Dillard, Kim, and Li found that the persuasiveness of anti-sugarsweetened beverage PSA messages calling for policy change using fear appeal could have been improved by 17% if reactance was eliminated [60].…”
Section: Persuasive Outcomes and Hypothesesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…W. Brehm’s (1966) and S. S. Brehm and Brehm’s (1981) psychological reactance process has been offered as an important cause for these unfortunate outcomes and remains a relevant theoretical framework for research on persuasive health communication (Dillard & Shen, 2005; Erceg-Hurn & Steed, 2011; Quick & Bates, 2010; Wagner, Howland, & Mann, 2015; Wong, Harrison, & Harvell, 2015).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%