1971
DOI: 10.3758/bf03336022
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Rat’s aversion to flavors following induced illness

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
6
0

Year Published

1973
1973
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
1
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Hence, Experiment 4 has segregated the two kinds of visceral discomforts (i.e., pain and nausea). In other words, visceral pain induced by hypertonic saline qualitatively differs from nausea induced by LiCl, although hypertonic saline was effective in causing taste avoidance as demonstrated here and by other researchers (e.g., Andrews & Braveman, 1975;Hargrave & Bolles, 1971;Lin et al, 2013;Sakai & Yamamoto, 1997).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 67%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Hence, Experiment 4 has segregated the two kinds of visceral discomforts (i.e., pain and nausea). In other words, visceral pain induced by hypertonic saline qualitatively differs from nausea induced by LiCl, although hypertonic saline was effective in causing taste avoidance as demonstrated here and by other researchers (e.g., Andrews & Braveman, 1975;Hargrave & Bolles, 1971;Lin et al, 2013;Sakai & Yamamoto, 1997).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…Experiment 4 was designed to examine this issue by exploring the effect of visceral pain on pica behavior. As noted in the introduction of this article, repeated injections of hypertonic saline evoke visceral pain and cause avoidance of paired taste (e.g., Hargrave & Bolles, 1971;Lin et al, 2013;Sakai & Yamamoto, 1997). Hence, in Experiment 4, pica behavior in rats was monitored to assess the effect of such injections.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…With increased experimental interest, the list of effective agents for producing taste aversions has been greatly expanded. Such diverse chemicals as d-amphetamine and mescaline (Cappell & Le Blanc, 1971), ethanol (Lester, Nachman, & Le Magnen, 1970), cyclophosphamide (Garcia, Ervin, & Koelling, 1967), hypertonic saline (Hargrave & Bolles, 1971), p-tetrahydrocannabinol (Elsmore & Fletcher, 1972), morphine and chlordiazepoxide (Cappell, Le Blanc, & Endrenyi, 1973), cyclohexamide (Booth & Simson, 1973), formalin (Woods, Weisinger, & Wald, 1971), methyl mercury (Braun & Snyder, 1973), anesthetics (Brown & Glusman, 1971), and even intravenously injected isotonic saline (Revusky, Smith, & Chalmers, 1971) have been shown to produce effective learned taste aversions.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, it has long been known that exteroceptive cues, suchas brightness of wallsand floortexture, can be associatedwith illness (e.g., Garcia, Kimeldorf, & Hunt, 1957). However, exteroceptive cues are not alwayseasily associatedwith illnessand are not typicallyas effective as tastes (Best, Best,& Henggeler, 1977;Best, Best,& Mickley, 1973;Garcia, Kimeldorf, & Hunt, 1961;Garcia & Koelling, 1966;Garcia, Kovner, & Green, 1970;Green, Holmstrom, & Wollman, 1974;Hargrave & Bolles, 1971;Larsen & Hyde, 1977;Slotnik, Brown, & Gelhard, 1977). Oral stimuli suchas dry versus wet (Garcia, Hankins, Robinson, & Vogt, 1972) and warm versus cool (Nachman, 1970) have also been effectively associated with illness.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%