2009
DOI: 10.1007/s11145-009-9199-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Rapid serial naming and reading ability: the role of lexical access

Abstract: Rapid serial naming tasks are frequently used to explain variance in reading skill. However, the construct being measured by rapid naming is yet undetermined. The Phonological Processing theory suggests that rapid naming relates to reading because of similar demands of access to long-term stored phonological representations of visual stimuli. Some researchers have argued that isolated or discrete-trial naming is a more precise measure of lexical access than serial naming, thus it is likely that any shared vari… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

10
76
0
5

Year Published

2013
2013
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 82 publications
(91 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
10
76
0
5
Order By: Relevance
“…With the provision of large to very large effect sizes for all reading proficiency levels, while cutting across age levels, our results are in line with the general consensus on the pivotal and enduring importance of RAN and PA to reading (dis)abilities (Kirby et al, 2008;Landerl et al, 2013;Logan, Schatschneider, & Wagner, 2011;Torppa et al, 2012;van den Bos, 2008). However, it should be noted that larger effects of both predictors were established for the most severely impaired readers.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…With the provision of large to very large effect sizes for all reading proficiency levels, while cutting across age levels, our results are in line with the general consensus on the pivotal and enduring importance of RAN and PA to reading (dis)abilities (Kirby et al, 2008;Landerl et al, 2013;Logan, Schatschneider, & Wagner, 2011;Torppa et al, 2012;van den Bos, 2008). However, it should be noted that larger effects of both predictors were established for the most severely impaired readers.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…In the RAN task, readers are required to name a series of familiar stimuli, such as letters, digits, colors, or objects. The RAN task is one significant indicator of development status in reading skill (Kirby, Parrila, & Pfeiffer, 2003; Logan, Schatschneider, & Wagner, 2011). Dyslexic children on average perform poorer than normal children in this task (see Norton & Wolf, 2012, for a review).…”
Section: Rapid Automatized Namingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other perspectives questioning the phonological view point out that although RAN obviously shares some characteristics with phonological skills , these characteristics are not sufficient to explain the RAN-reading relationship (Jones, Branigan, Hatzidaki, & ObregĂłn, 2010;Powell et al, 2007), and that RAN continues to predict reading after controlling for phonological awareness and/or phonological short-term memory (Bowers et al, 1988;Kirby et al, 2003;Parrila et al, 2004). In addition, results in which RAN predicted reading performance better than discrete naming (i.e., a more classical measure of lexical access) were interpreted to mean that RAN cannot be subsumed under lexical access of phonological stimuli (Logan, Schatschneider, & Wagner, 2011).While each of the perspectives presented above have been both supported and criticized, the question of what underlies the connection between RAN and reading continues to await a comprehensive answer. In studies exploring these viewpoints, while it has been shown that RAN shares a significant part of its predictive variance on reading fluency with processing speed, phonological awareness, phonological short-term memory, letter knowledge, and orthographic processing, RAN survives as a predictor of reading fluency even after controlling for these variables Georgiou, Papadopoulos, Fella, & Parrila, 2012;Georgiou, Tziraki et al, 2013;Poulsen et al, 2015; for a review, see .…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other perspectives questioning the phonological view point out that although RAN obviously shares some characteristics with phonological skills , these characteristics are not sufficient to explain the RAN-reading relationship (Jones, Branigan, Hatzidaki, & ObregĂłn, 2010;Powell et al, 2007), and that RAN continues to predict reading after controlling for phonological awareness and/or phonological short-term memory (Bowers et al, 1988;Kirby et al, 2003;Parrila et al, 2004). In addition, results in which RAN predicted reading performance better than discrete naming (i.e., a more classical measure of lexical access) were interpreted to mean that RAN cannot be subsumed under lexical access of phonological stimuli (Logan, Schatschneider, & Wagner, 2011).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%