2013
DOI: 10.1080/10888438.2012.753452
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does Training in Syllable Recognition Improve Reading Speed? A Computer-Based Trial With Poor Readers From Second and Third Grade

Abstract: This research evaluated the ability to retrieve and fluently name serially presented familiar items, known as rapid automatized naming (RAN), as an underlying skill in reading disability and its role in a reading fluency intervention. The specificity of RAN on reading was examined in two studies among children with learning difficulties. These two studies showed that RAN was most strongly related to reading disabilities (RD), and less clearly related to math or attention deficits. This was especially true when… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
39
1
7

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 73 publications
(48 citation statements)
references
References 205 publications
1
39
1
7
Order By: Relevance
“…However, since then a number of computer interventions with non-English speakers have been found to be very effective. These include Finnish (Saine, Lerkkanen, Ahonen, Tolvanen & Lyytinen, 2010;Heikkila, Mikko, Narhi, Westerholm & Ahonen, 2013;Lyytinen, Ronimus, Alanko, Poikkeus & Taanila, 2007), Spanish (Jiménez et al, 2007) and French (Ecalle, Kleinz & Magnan, 2013;Magnan & Ecalle, 2006). Because these non-English languages have more consistent relations between graphemes and phonemes, this may explain their effectiveness in contrast to computer interventions with Englishspeaking children which face the challenge of inconsistent grapheme-phoneme relationships.…”
Section: Abbreviationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, since then a number of computer interventions with non-English speakers have been found to be very effective. These include Finnish (Saine, Lerkkanen, Ahonen, Tolvanen & Lyytinen, 2010;Heikkila, Mikko, Narhi, Westerholm & Ahonen, 2013;Lyytinen, Ronimus, Alanko, Poikkeus & Taanila, 2007), Spanish (Jiménez et al, 2007) and French (Ecalle, Kleinz & Magnan, 2013;Magnan & Ecalle, 2006). Because these non-English languages have more consistent relations between graphemes and phonemes, this may explain their effectiveness in contrast to computer interventions with Englishspeaking children which face the challenge of inconsistent grapheme-phoneme relationships.…”
Section: Abbreviationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because the intervention did not focus on speed-related tasks, the improvements might have been because children became more efficient at accessing phonological material and/or translating visual stimuli into a phonological form. It is also worth noting that Torgesen et al (2010) have reported positive effects of a computer intervention on naming speed with children in the United States, however, Heikkila et al (2013) failed to produce a similar effect with Finnish children.…”
Section: Effectiveness Of a Computer-assisted Interventionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The electroencephalogram (EEG) data collected in the study suggest that GraphoGame use also may be beneficial in modulating the neural basis of phonetic discrimination. Heikkilä et al (2013) found that a brief intervention with GraphoGame increased children's reading speed of the syllables encountered in the game.…”
Section: The Graphogame Learning Environmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research has shown that digital games can enhance children's learning, with positive effects found, for example, in the development of reading skills (e.g., Heikkilä, Aro, Närhi, Westerholm, & Ahonen, 2013;Magnan & Ecalle, 2006;Saine, Lerkkanen, Ahonen, Tolvanen, & Lyytinen, 2010, 2011 and mathematical skills (e.g., Miller & Robertson, 2011;Shin, Sutherland, Norris, & Soloway, 2012). Additionally, children usually are very eager to use digital learning games (e.g., Rosas et al, 2003;Tüzün, Yılmaz-Soylu, Karakuş, İnal, & Kızılkaya, 2009).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In these transparent languages, one focus has been on speeded sublexical reading and its relation to reading fluency. Training studies have focused on identification of syllables (e.g., Heikkilä, Aro, Närhi, Westerholm, and Ahonen, 2013;Huemer, Aro, Landerl, and Lyytinen, 2010) and onset consonant-clusters (e.g., Hintikka, Landerl, Aro, and Lyytinen, 2008;Thaler, Ebner, Wimmer, and Landerl, 2004), and have examined letter cluster versus individual letter training (e.g., Marinus, de Jong, and van der Leij, 2012). Following relatively short, speeded training with these various sublexical units presented in isolation or within words, studies have generally found increased rate of reading both trained and untrained words containing these clusters (e.g., Hintikka et al, 2008;Thaler et al, 2004).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%