2020
DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/97nhj
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Punitive but discerning: Reputation can fuel ambiguously-deserved punishment, but does not erode sensitivity to nuance

Abstract: Reputation concerns can motivate moralistic punishment, but existing evidence comes exclusively from contexts in which punishment is unambiguously deserved. Recent debates surrounding “virtue signaling” and “outrage culture” raise the question of whether reputation may also fuel punishment in more ambiguous cases—and even encourage indiscriminate punishment that ignores moral nuance. But when the moral case for punishment is ambiguous, do people actually expect punishing to make them look good? And if so, are … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
2

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
(80 reference statements)
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Prior research has begun to shed light on these discrepant findings by characterizing how beliefs about authorities' motivations affect how people change their beliefs about wrongness of the act (Tyler, 2006;Mulder, 2009;Verboon & van Dijke, 2011), and vice versa (J. Jordan & Kteily, 2020;Sarin, Ho, Martin, & Cushman, 2021). These studies typically focus on one component of these inferences at a time, and do not consider the interplay between them.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Prior research has begun to shed light on these discrepant findings by characterizing how beliefs about authorities' motivations affect how people change their beliefs about wrongness of the act (Tyler, 2006;Mulder, 2009;Verboon & van Dijke, 2011), and vice versa (J. Jordan & Kteily, 2020;Sarin, Ho, Martin, & Cushman, 2021). These studies typically focus on one component of these inferences at a time, and do not consider the interplay between them.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Consequently, many studies have documented the positive perceptions that observers have of people who are willing to punish: in both economic games and vignette studies, third-parties who choose to punish are judged as more trustworthy and less selfish (J. J. Jordan & Rand, 2020), more competent and and more moral (Gordon, Madden, & Lea, 2014;Gordon & Lea, 2016;Dhaliwal, Skarlicki, Hoegg, & Daniels, 2020;de Kwaadsteniet, Kiyonari, Molenmaker, & van Dijk, 2019;J. Jordan & Kteily, 2020;Tsai, Trinh, & Liu, 2022), and are more likely to be chosen as cooperation partners than people who choose not to punish the same transgression.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…"Good" group members punish wrongdoers and adopt worldviews that align with the group (Boggio et al, 2023;Henrich et al, 2006;Jordan & Kteily, 2020), which on the surface suggests normative influence-they meet the group's expectations and signal that they are a cooperative group member (Curry et al, 2019). In turn, group members who follow and enforce norms often gain social status within the group (Ellemers et al, 2008;Grubbs et al, 2019).…”
Section: Why Moral Expressions Are Influenced By the Social Environmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such punishment can be socially rewarded ( 19 ) and signal trustworthiness ( 20 25 ) by conveying that the punisher is unlikely to themself transgress. Consequently, people punish more when their decisions are observable to others ( 26 , 27 ) and when punishment has greater signaling value ( 20 , 21 ).…”
Section: Does Reputation Drive People To Punish Without Looking?mentioning
confidence: 99%