2005
DOI: 10.1016/j.heares.2004.11.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Psychophysical evidence for adaptation of central auditory processors for interaural differences in time and level

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
5

Citation Types

11
77
1

Year Published

2008
2008
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 59 publications
(89 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
11
77
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The first question raised by these experiments is whether the aftereffect reflects a criterion shift at the decision stage or true sensory adaptation. Against the criterion interpretation, Kashino and Nishida (1998), Phillips and Hall (2005), and Vigneault-MacLean et al (2007) showed that the aftereffect is frequency-specific: that is, preceding sound and test tone must share the same frequency region for the aftereffect to occur. The second question is whether the effect of the preceding sound is location-or cue-specific.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The first question raised by these experiments is whether the aftereffect reflects a criterion shift at the decision stage or true sensory adaptation. Against the criterion interpretation, Kashino and Nishida (1998), Phillips and Hall (2005), and Vigneault-MacLean et al (2007) showed that the aftereffect is frequency-specific: that is, preceding sound and test tone must share the same frequency region for the aftereffect to occur. The second question is whether the effect of the preceding sound is location-or cue-specific.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…After prolonged exposure to a preceding sound at a fixed location, an auditory spatial aftereffect can be observed (Thurlow and Jack 1973;Kashino and Nishida 1998;Dong et al 1999;Carlile et al 2000;Phillips and Hall 2005;Vigneault-MacLean et al 2007). In all of these studies, the apparent location of the test sound is shifted away from the preceding sound's position.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In vision research, this approach was used to investigate neurons tuned to colour, motion, spatial frequency and orientation, to name but a few stimulus dimensions (for reviews see Clifford et al, 2007;Webster, 2011). Similarly, adaptation has been extensively employed in auditory (Carlile, Hyams, & Delaney, 2001;Kay & Matthews, 1972;Phillips & Hall, 2005), somatosensory (Hahn, 1966;Hollins, Sliman, & Washburn, 2001;Miyazaki, Yamamoto, Uchida, & Kitazawa, 2006), and olfactory research (Dalton, 2000;. Despite its widespread use, there are some limitations to this paradigm.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In part, the model is based on neurophysiological observations of the spatial sensitivity of cortical neurons: Of the neurons sensitive to source location, most are broadly tuned to the contralateral acoustic hemifield, with smaller proportions tuned to the ipsilateral hemifield or to near-midline azimuths (Stecker et al, 2005;Lee and Middlebrooks, 2013;Zhou and Wang, 2012). In concurrent psychophysical studies, Phillips and Hall (2005) and Dingle et al (2012Dingle et al ( , 2013) studied human sound lateralization judgments for dichotic tones lateralized on the basis of interaural time (ITD) or level differences (ILDs). Using selective adaptation paradigms, they provided evidence that intracranial azimuth judgments are based on the relative outputs of three neural-perceptual channels with broad tuning to the left and right acoustic hemifields, and to the midline.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%