2012
DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2011.651091
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prospective memory reminders: A laboratory investigation of initiation source and age effects

Abstract: Prior research indicates that, in some circumstances, reminders may facilitate prospective remembering. However, it remains unclear whether this effect is dependent on the initiation source (self vs. external), whether it is moderated by task type (event vs. time based), or whether the provision of standardized reminders particularly benefits older adults. In the current study, young (n = 48) and older adults (n = 47) were tested on a laboratory-based prospective memory task in which they encountered three cou… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

4
37
2

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 53 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
4
37
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Of particular interest was the finding that a cue-driven monitoring process was directly initiated by subtle cues, in the form of semantically related images that were embedded in the continuous task. Past studies have revealed that contextual cues in the environment often prompt retrieval of a future intention (Harris & Wilkins, 1982, Henry et al, 2012Kuhlmann & Rummel, 2014;Kvavilashvilli & Fisher, 2007), and the present research extended this finding with the observation that contextual cues can trigger retrieval of the future intention, thereby prompting participants to overtly monitor for prospective memory targets. Salient external cues have been shown to be helpful to adults (Guajardo & Best, 2000) in a variety of laboratory studies (Einstein & McDaniel, 1996;Kim & Mayhorn, 2008;Villa, 1998), including investigations of air traffic control (Loft et al, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 65%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Of particular interest was the finding that a cue-driven monitoring process was directly initiated by subtle cues, in the form of semantically related images that were embedded in the continuous task. Past studies have revealed that contextual cues in the environment often prompt retrieval of a future intention (Harris & Wilkins, 1982, Henry et al, 2012Kuhlmann & Rummel, 2014;Kvavilashvilli & Fisher, 2007), and the present research extended this finding with the observation that contextual cues can trigger retrieval of the future intention, thereby prompting participants to overtly monitor for prospective memory targets. Salient external cues have been shown to be helpful to adults (Guajardo & Best, 2000) in a variety of laboratory studies (Einstein & McDaniel, 1996;Kim & Mayhorn, 2008;Villa, 1998), including investigations of air traffic control (Loft et al, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 65%
“…Both self-report and behavioral data suggest that environmental cues can serve as a reminder for a prospective memory intention (Harris & Wilkins, 1982;Henry, Rendell, Phillips, Dunlop, & Kliegel, 2012;Kvavilashvili & Fisher, 2007) and that environmental cues can be used to support prospective memory in laboratory simulated air traffic control scenarios (Loft, Smith, & Bhaskara, 2011). It remains unclear exactly how environmental cues interact with the cognitive mechanisms supporting prospective memory, although some research has suggested that people will adjust how they allocate attention to the prospective memory task if they are instructed about contextual cues that will accompany the prospective memory target (Kuhlmann & Rummel, 2014).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Participants move around the board with the roll of a dice. The time of the virtual day is displayed on a virtual time clock calibrated to the position of the token on the board (every 2 squares the time increased 15 minutes) (e.g., Griffiths et al, 2012;Henry et al, 2012;Mioni et al, 2013;Rendell et al, 2011). As participants circuit the board, they have decisions to make and things to remember to perform.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Virtual Week is a very promising task for investigating PM performance and it has been extensively used with normal ageing (Aberle, Rendell, Rose, McDaniel, & Kliegel, 2010;Henry, Rendell, Phillips, Dunlop, & Kliegel, 2012;Margrett, Reese-Melancon, & Rendell, 2011;Rendell & Craik, 2000;Rose, Rendell, McDaniel, Aberle, & Kliegel, 2010;Rendell et al, 2011) and different clinical populations: those with abnormal ageing Thompson et al, 2010;Will et al, 2009), patients with multiple sclerosis (Kardiasmenos, Clawson, Wilken, & Wallin, 2008;Rendell, Jensen et al, 2007a;West et al, 2007), schizophrenics (Henry, Rendell, Kliegel, & Altgassen, 2007;Henry, Rendell, Rogers, Altgassen, & Kliegel, 2011), substance users (Leitz, Morgan, Bisby, Rendell, & Curran, 2009;Paraskevaides et al, 2010;Rendell, Gray, Henry, & Tolan, 2007;Rendell, Mazur, & Henry, 2009), Parkinson's patients (Foster et al, 2013), and patients with brain damage (Kim, Craik, Luo, & Ween, 2009;Mioni et al, 2013). Virtual Week has been shown to be sensitive to PM deficits with each of these groups: normal ageing, abnormal ageing and the various clinical groups.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is of practical significance because external artifacts and reminders can be effective tools for remembering delayed intentions (Allen, 2002;Henry, Rendell, Phillips, Dunlop, & Kliegel, 2012;Heylighen & Vidal, 2008;Maylor, 1990). If we are to understand how intentions are fulfilled in everyday life it is therefore important to understand how we decide whether or not to set reminders, and what interventions might affect these decisions.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%