2016
DOI: 10.1007/s10683-016-9488-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Promises and lies: can observers detect deception in written messages

Abstract: We design a laboratory experiment to examine predictions of trustworthiness in a novel three-person trust game. We investigate whether and why observers of the game can predict the trustworthiness of hand-written communications. Observers report their perception of the trustworthiness of messages, and make predictions about the senders’ behavior. Using observers’ decisions, we are able to classify messages as “promises” or “empty talk.” Drawing from substantial previous research, we hypothesize that certain fa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
18
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
1
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We and disregard this aspect in their analyses; similar conclusions can be found in Chen and Houser (2017).…”
supporting
confidence: 62%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We and disregard this aspect in their analyses; similar conclusions can be found in Chen and Houser (2017).…”
supporting
confidence: 62%
“…Other characteristics of messages may also matter. Using a modied two-person trust game with an additional external observer, Chen and Houser (2017) report that other important features of a written message are its length (which increases trust) and the fact of mentioning money (which is associated with untrustworthiness).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…(That is, participants’ decisions may be affected by their conjectures about future additional surprise rule changes.) We acknowledge this criticism, but note that this technique is not unique to our study (see, for example, the “surprise message opportunity” in Chen and Houser’s [ 60 ] double-message treatment). Also, without such a “surprise”, it would be difficult to argue that proposers in this treatment and those in our baseline treatment faced similar conditions.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…and Van den Assem et al (2012) analyzea dataset from a high-stake prisoners' dilemma game played in a TV show that females are more likely to share the prize than males, that external observers can accurately predict this gender difference Bicchieri et al (2010),. in turn, find no statistical evidence for gender differences in trust and trustworthiness in a trust game played with different modes of communication (either computer-mediated or face-to-face), and disregard this aspect in their analyses; similar conclusions can be found inChen and Houser (2017).…”
mentioning
confidence: 89%
“…Other characteristics may also matter. Using a modified two-person trust game with an additional external observer, Chen and Houser (2017) report that other important features of a written message are its length (which increases trust) and the fact of mentioning money (which is associated with untrustworthiness). Factors such as the use of encompassing words (like "we" or "us"), or the sender's gender, do not seem to matter.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%