2014
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-014-0714-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Processes of incremental message planning during conversation

Abstract: Speaking begins with the formulation of an intended preverbal message and linguistic encoding of this information. The transition from thought to speech occurs incrementally, with cascading planning at subsequent levels of production. In this article, we aim to specify the mechanisms that support incremental message preparation. We contrast two hypotheses about the mechanisms responsible for incorporating message-level information into a linguistic plan. According to the Initial Preparation view, messages can … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
11
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
3
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
1
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Such a scenario is compatible with the considerable amount of evidence pointing to the incremental nature of sentence production (e.g., Meyer, 1996;Schriefers et al, 1998;van de Velde et al, 2014;Brown-Schmidt and Konopka, 2015;Zhao and Yang, 2016) as initially hypothesized by Kempen and Hoenkamp (1987). According to this, speakers do not necessarily wait for all the bits and pieces of a phrase or sentence to have been processed and set in their places before they start uttering it.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 64%
“…Such a scenario is compatible with the considerable amount of evidence pointing to the incremental nature of sentence production (e.g., Meyer, 1996;Schriefers et al, 1998;van de Velde et al, 2014;Brown-Schmidt and Konopka, 2015;Zhao and Yang, 2016) as initially hypothesized by Kempen and Hoenkamp (1987). According to this, speakers do not necessarily wait for all the bits and pieces of a phrase or sentence to have been processed and set in their places before they start uttering it.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 64%
“…This situation should afford a stronger learning opportunity than in comprehension, where the input signal and the message that the comprehender gleans from it unfold over time. Language production also requires the generation of an utterance plan, and because planning precedes execution by some time, planning entails maintaining information in working memory (Brown-Schmidt & Konopka, 2015). Indeed, MacDonald (2016) argued that the utterance plan is the maintenance portion of verbal working memory.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Turning to production, we start by noting the powerful intuition that we speak "into the void"that is, that we plan only a short distance ahead. Indeed, experimental studies suggest that, for example, when producing an utterance involving several noun phrases, people plan just one (Smith & Wheeldon 1999), or perhaps two, noun phrases ahead (Konopka 2012), and they can modify a message during production in the light of new perceptual input (Brown-Schmidt & Konopka 2015). Moreover, speecherror data (e.g., Cutler 1982) reveal that, across representational levels, errors tend to be highly local: Phonological, morphemic, and syntactic errors apply to neighboring chunks within each level (where material may be moved, swapped, or deleted).…”
Section: Implications Of Strategy 1: Incremental Processingmentioning
confidence: 99%