2015
DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x1500031x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Now-or-Never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language

Abstract: Memory is fleeting. New material rapidly obliterates previous material. How, then, can the brain deal successfully with the continual deluge of linguistic input? We argue that, to deal with this "Now-or-Never" bottleneck, the brain must compress and recode linguistic input as rapidly as possible. This observation has strong implications for the nature of language processing: (1) the language system must "eagerly" recode and compress linguistic input; (2) as the bottleneck recurs at each new representational le… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

24
422
4

Year Published

2015
2015
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 466 publications
(487 citation statements)
references
References 324 publications
(216 reference statements)
24
422
4
Order By: Relevance
“…However, in the case of large quantities of sequential data, where all items may be equally important, proximity of repeated occurrences in time and frequency of recurrence are the best first-resort tests of meaningfulness. The selective pressure of language in the direction of smaller buffer sizes and moderate fixation rates may explain why people are not better at memorizing sequential data verbatim (58), which would require larger buffer sizes and more rapid fixation.…”
Section: The Case Of Human Language and Memory Constraintsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, in the case of large quantities of sequential data, where all items may be equally important, proximity of repeated occurrences in time and frequency of recurrence are the best first-resort tests of meaningfulness. The selective pressure of language in the direction of smaller buffer sizes and moderate fixation rates may explain why people are not better at memorizing sequential data verbatim (58), which would require larger buffer sizes and more rapid fixation.…”
Section: The Case Of Human Language and Memory Constraintsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…[Recall our earlier discussion of Christiansen and Chater's memory bottleneck (58).] We assume that a larger buffer that can accommodate more data can evolve, but whether or not it will be adaptive depends on the kind of computations needed to process the data in the buffer.…”
Section: The Case Of Human Language and Memory Constraintsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Struggling to spell a word-regardless of whether the word is then spelled correctly-risks damage to higher level, conceptual, rhetorical or even syntactic structures in the text. Language processing operates under a stringent "now-or-never" constraint (Christiansen & Chater, 2016). If spelling is attention demanding, and therefore draws processing resources away from higher-level processes, the writer may, in a literal sense, forget what they were going to say.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such duality is characteristic of all perceptual coding and recoding and thus always multi-representational to ensure the accuracy of signal acquisition (Christiansen & Chater, 2015). Because the melodic parametric scale is registrally triplex in nature, any reversal of registral direction always entails the activation of an implication in the reversed direction (whether down, up, or lateral).…”
Section: Combinations and Chains: Double Functioningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The goal-note of the third degree is stable but slightly less so than the fifth degree). Structural tones, in other words, are ''lossy'' cognitive compressions recoding and integrating lower-level detail (Christiansen & Chater, 2015).…”
Section: An Analytical Examplementioning
confidence: 99%