2017
DOI: 10.1002/ece3.3165
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Predation risk and abiotic habitat parameters affect personality traits in extremophile populations of a neotropical fish (Poecilia vivipara)

Abstract: Understanding whether and how ambient ecological conditions affect the distribution of personality types within and among populations lies at the heart of research on animal personality. Several studies have focussed on only one agent of divergent selection (or driver of plastic changes in behavior), considering either predation risk or a single abiotic ecological factor. Here, we investigated how an array of abiotic and biotic environmental factors simultaneously shape population differences in boldness, acti… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
17
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 77 publications
(158 reference statements)
0
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Populations of P. vivipara at either extreme of the salinity gradient differ significantly in traits such as body size and shape, fecundity, and reproductive allotment, which are likely determined genetically [38] [39]. Poecilia vivipara also varies markedly in personality traits including boldness, activity, and sociability/shoaling behaviour, which are modulated by environmental factors such as water transparency/visibility, salinity, and dissolved oxygen [40]. Variability in personality traits vanishes, however, under homogenous laboratory conditions, indicating that plasticity underlies the behavioural differences observed in nature [40].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Populations of P. vivipara at either extreme of the salinity gradient differ significantly in traits such as body size and shape, fecundity, and reproductive allotment, which are likely determined genetically [38] [39]. Poecilia vivipara also varies markedly in personality traits including boldness, activity, and sociability/shoaling behaviour, which are modulated by environmental factors such as water transparency/visibility, salinity, and dissolved oxygen [40]. Variability in personality traits vanishes, however, under homogenous laboratory conditions, indicating that plasticity underlies the behavioural differences observed in nature [40].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Poecilia vivipara also varies markedly in personality traits including boldness, activity, and sociability/shoaling behaviour, which are modulated by environmental factors such as water transparency/visibility, salinity, and dissolved oxygen [40]. Variability in personality traits vanishes, however, under homogenous laboratory conditions, indicating that plasticity underlies the behavioural differences observed in nature [40]. The available data thus suggest that genetics and plasticity play fundamental roles in the distribution of ecological and behavioural traits of P. vivipara, which are potentially relevant for the colonization of heterogeneous environments subjected to climatic fluctuations.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In particular, predation pressure is regarded as a major environmental factor which may strongly influence the development of consistent inter-individual differences in behaviour (e.g. [2931], but see [32]). Based on the above-stated findings and considerations, it seems that many different cognitive styles can emerge depending on the precise ecological circumstances in which individuals live.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Poecilia mexicana (including the population studied here) has repeatedly been characterized for risk-taking behaviour, and previous studies reported high behavioural repeatability, with R-values ranging between 0.53 and 0.64 ( freezing time after a simulated predator attack , R = 0.64, Sommer-Trembo et al, 2016a; repeatability across time to emerge from shelter and freezing time after a simulated predator attack , R = 0.53, Sommer-Trembo & Plath, 2018). Slightly lower, yet significant R-values were reported for the related guppy ( time to emerge from shelter , R = 0.51, Brown & Irving, 2013; time to emerge from shelter , R = 0.51 for females and R = 0.36 for males, Irving & Brown, 2013; time to emerge from shelter , R = 0.33, White, Kells & Wilson, 2016) and other poeciliid fishes (e.g., Gambusia affinis, time to emerge from shelter , R = 0.29 in Cote, Fogarty, Weinersmith, Brodin & Sih, 2010 and R = 0.39 in Gomes-Silva, Liu, Chen, Plath & Sommer-Trembo, 2017; Poecilia vivipara, time to emerge from shelter , R = 0.70, Sommer-Trembo et al 2016b). For our present study, we initially screened a large number of potential stimulus and focal individuals so as to be able to select stimulus pairs with contrasting behavioural type (see methods).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%